An Early Years Toolbox for Assessing Early Executive Function, Language, Self-Regulation, and Social Development: Validity, Reliability, and Preliminary Norms

Steven J Howard, Edward Melhuish, Steven J Howard, Edward Melhuish

Abstract

Several methods of assessing executive function (EF), self-regulation, language development, and social development in young children have been developed over previous decades. Yet new technologies make available methods of assessment not previously considered. In resolving conceptual and pragmatic limitations of existing tools, the Early Years Toolbox (EYT) offers substantial advantages for early assessment of language, EF, self-regulation, and social development. In the current study, results of our large-scale administration of this toolbox to 1,764 preschool and early primary school students indicated very good reliability, convergent validity with existing measures, and developmental sensitivity. Results were also suggestive of better capture of children's emerging abilities relative to comparison measures. Preliminary norms are presented, showing a clear developmental trajectory across half-year age groups. The accessibility of the EYT, as well as its advantages over existing measures, offers considerably enhanced opportunities for objective measurement of young children's abilities to enable research and educational applications.

Keywords: development; early years; executive function; language; self-regulation; social.

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Screen captures of Early Years Toolbox Apps (title screen and test item screen captures). Note. Top row, from left to right: Mr. Ant (visual-spatial working memory), Not This (phonological working memory), Card Sorting (shifting), Go/No-Go (inhibition). Bottom row, from left to right: Expressive Vocabulary, Child Self-Regulation and Behavior Questionnaire (self-regulation and social development).

References

    1. Adi-Japha E., Klein P. S. (2009). Relations between parenting quality and cognitive performance of children experiencing various amounts of childcare. Child Development, 80, 803-906.
    1. Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2012). Census Fact Sheet [Fact Sheet]. Retrieved from
    1. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2012). A picture of Australia’s children 2012. Retrieved from
    1. Berk L. E., Mann T. D., Ogan A. T. (2006). Make-believe play: Wellspring for development of self–regulation. In Singer D. G., Golinkoff R. M., Hirsh-Pasek K. (Eds.), Play = Learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp. 74-100). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    1. Best J. R., Miller P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641-1660.
    1. Best J. R., Miller P. H., Jones L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29, 180-200.
    1. Blair C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111-127.
    1. Blair C., Diamond A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 899-911.
    1. Blair C., Razza R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647-663.
    1. Blair C., Zelazo P. D., Greenberg M. T. (2005). The measurement of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 561-571.
    1. Bodrova E., Leong D. (2007). Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian approach to early childhood education (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
    1. Boeckaerts M., Corno L. (2005). Self-regulation in the classroom: A perspective on assessment and intervention. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 54, 199-231.
    1. Bull R., Espy K. A., Wiebe S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205-228.
    1. Carlson S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595-616.
    1. Case R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York, NY: Academic Press.
    1. Chi M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge students and memory development. In Siegler R. (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 73-96). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    1. Crone E. A., Dahl R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 13, 636-650.
    1. Daglar M., Melhuish E., Barnes J. (2011). Parenting and pre-school child behaviour amongst Turkish immigrant, migrant and non-migrant families. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 261-279.
    1. Diamond A., Lee K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959-964.
    1. Dickinson D. K., D. K. P. O. (Eds.). (2001). Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
    1. Duncan G. J., Dowsett C. J., Claessens A., Magnuson K., Huston A. C., Klebanov P., . . . Japel C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428-1446.
    1. Elliott C. D., Smith P., McCulloch K. (1996). British Ability Scales second edition (BAS II): Administration and scoring manual. London, England: Nelson.
    1. Fenson L., Dale P., Reznick J. S., Bates E., Thal D., Pethick S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1-185.
    1. Friedman N. P., Miyake A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101-135.
    1. Galinsky E. (2010). Mind in the making. New York, NY: Harper Collins.
    1. Goodman R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586.
    1. Goodman R., Meltzer H., Bailey V. (1998). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 125-130.
    1. Hackman D. A., Farah M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 65-73.
    1. Hart B., Risley R. T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
    1. Hoff E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1378.
    1. Hoff-Ginsberg E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782-796.
    1. Hofmann W., Schmeichel B. J., Baddeley A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174-180.
    1. Hogan A. E., Scott K. G., Bauer C. R. (1992). The Adaptive Social Behavior Inventory (ASBI): A new assessment of social competence in high-risk three-year-olds. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 10, 230-239.
    1. Howard S. J., Johnson J., Pascual-Leone J. (2014). Clarifying inhibitory control: Diversity and development of attentional inhibition. Cognitive Development, 31, 1-21.
    1. Howard S. J., Okely A. D. (2015). Catching fish and avoiding sharks: Investigating factors that influence developmentally appropriate measurement of preschoolers’ inhibitory control. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 33(6), 585-596.
    1. Hughes C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233-253.
    1. Hughes C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
    1. Huttenlocher J., Vasilyeva M., Waterfall H., Vevea J., Hedges L. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1062-1083.
    1. Im-Bolter N., Johnson J., Pascual-Leone J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 1822-1841.
    1. Kochanska G., Murray K. T., Harlan E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220-232.
    1. Korkman M. (1999). Applying Luria’s diagnostic principles in the neuropsychological assessment of children. Neuropsychological Review, 9, 89-105.
    1. Korkman M., Kirk U., Kemp S. (2007). NEPSY-II: Clinical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
    1. Lamb M. E., Ahnert L. (2006). Nonparental child care: Context, concepts, correlates, and consequences. In Damon W., Lerner R. M., Renninger K. A., Sigek I. E. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 4: Child psychology in practice (5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
    1. Lehto J. E., Juujärvi P., Kooistra L., Pulkkinen L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59-80.
    1. Lillard A. S., Lerner M. D., Hopkins E. J., Dore R. A., Smith E. D., Palmquist C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1-34.
    1. Lugo-Gil J., Tamis-LeMonda C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting quality: Links to children’s cognitive development across the first three years. Child Development, 79, 1065-1085.
    1. McClelland M. M., Acock A. C., Morrison F. J. (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 471-490.
    1. McClelland M. M., Cameron C. E., Connor C. M., Farris C., Jewkes A., Morrison F. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43, 947-959.
    1. Melhuish E., Barnes J. (2012). Preschool programs for the general population. In Tremblay R. E., Boivin M., Peters R. V. (Eds.), Encyclopedia on early childhood development. Montreal, Quebec, Canada: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development and Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development; Retrieved from
    1. Melhuish E., Sylva K., Sammons P., Siraj-Blatchford I., Taggart B., Phan M. (2007). The home learning environment, attainment and resilience in Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 (EPPE 3-11). In Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Team (Eds.), Promoting equality in the early years: Report to the equalities review (pp. 50-71). London, England: Cabinet Office.
    1. Miech R., Essex M. J., Goldsmith H. H. (2001). Socioeconomic status and the adjustment to school: The role of self-regulation during early childhood. Sociology of Education, 74(2), 102-120.
    1. Miller M. R., Giesbrecht G. F., Müller U., McInerney R. J., Kerns K. A. (2012). A latent variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 395-423.
    1. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and the contributions to complete “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.
    1. Moffitt T. E., Arseneault L., Belsky J., Dickson N., Hancox R. J., Harrington H., . . . Caspi A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 2693-2698.
    1. Morra S. (1994). Issues in working memory measurement: Testing for M capacity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 143-159.
    1. National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB). (2013). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 1-172.
    1. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 976-1005.
    1. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Predicting individual differences in attention, memory, and planning in first graders from experiences at home, child care, and school. Developmental Psychology, 41, 99-114.
    1. Pan B. A., Rowe M. L., Singer J. D., Snow C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763-782.
    1. Pascual-Leone J., Johnson J. (2005). A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence. In Wilhelm O., Engle R. W. (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 177-201). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
    1. Pascual-Leone J., Johnson J. (2011). A developmental theory of mental attention: Its application to measurement and task analysis. In Barrouillet P., Gaillard V. (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian theories and cognitive approaches (pp. 13-46). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    1. Ponitz C. C., McClelland M. M., Matthews J. S., Morrison F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioural self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45, 605-619.
    1. Reese E., Sparks A., Leyva D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children’s language and emergent literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10, 97-117.
    1. Rhoades B. L., Greenberg M. T., Lanza S. T., Blair C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 638-662.
    1. Riggs N. R., Jahromi L. B., Razza R. P., Dillworth-Bart J. E., Müeller U. (2006). Executive function and the promotion of social-emotional competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 300-309.
    1. Rowe M. L., Raudenbush S. W., Goldin-Meadow S. (2012). The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Development, 83, 508-525.
    1. Sammons P., Elliot K., Sylva K., Melhuish E. C., Siraj-Blatchford I., Taggart B. (2004). The impact of pre-school on young children’s cognitive attainments at entry to reception. British Educational Research Journal, 30, 691-712.
    1. Shonkoff J. P., Phillips D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
    1. Sirois S., Spratling M., Thomas M. S. C., Westermann G., Mareschal D., Johnson M. H. (2008). Précis of neuroconstructivism: How the brain constructs cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 321-331.
    1. Slaughter V., Imuta K., Peterson C., Henry J. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86, 1159-1174.
    1. Snow C. E. (2011). The potential of discussion to improve reading comprehension. New York, NY: Lecture IDA.
    1. Snow C. E., Burns S., Griffin P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
    1. St. Clair-Thompson H. L., Gathercole S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745-759.
    1. Tulsky D. S., Carlozzi N. E., Chevalier N., Espy K. A., Beaumont J. L., Mungas D. (2013). V. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring working memory. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 70-87.
    1. Vandell D. L., Belsky J., Burchinal M., Steinberg L., Vandergrift N., &NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Child Development, 81, 737-756.
    1. Van der Ven S. H. G., Kroesbergen E. K., Boom J., Leseman P. P. M. (2012). The development of executive functions and early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 100-119.
    1. Weintraub S., Bauer P. J., Zelazo P. D., Wallner-Allen K., Dikmen S. S., Heaton R. K., . . . Gershon R. C. (2013). I. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Introduction and pediatric data. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 1-15.
    1. Wellman R. L., Lewis B. A., Freebairn L. A., Avrich A. A., Hansen A. J., Stein C. M. (2011). Narrative ability of children with speech sound disorders and the prediction of later literacy skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 561-579.
    1. Welsh J. A., Nix R. L., Blair C., Bierman K. L., Nelson K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 43-53.
    1. Wiebe S. A., Sheffield T. D., Espy K. A. (2012). Separating the fish from the sharks: A longitudinal study of preschool response inhibition. Child Development, 83, 1245-1261.
    1. Zelazo P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297-301.
    1. Zelazo P. D., Anderson J. E., Richler J., Wallner-Allen K., Beaumont J. L., Weintraub S. (2013). II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 16-33.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe