Effects of oxytocin and prosocial behavior on brain responses to direct and vicariously experienced pain

Tania Singer, Romana Snozzi, Geoffrey Bird, Predrag Petrovic, Giorgia Silani, Markus Heinrichs, Raymond J Dolan, Tania Singer, Romana Snozzi, Geoffrey Bird, Predrag Petrovic, Giorgia Silani, Markus Heinrichs, Raymond J Dolan

Abstract

In this study, we tested the validity of 2 popular assumptions about empathy: (a) empathy can be enhanced by oxytocin, a neuropeptide known to be crucial in affiliative behavior, and (b) individual differences in prosocial behavior are positively associated with empathic brain responses. To do so, we measured brain activity in a double-blind placebo-controlled study of 20 male participants either receiving painful stimulation to their own hand (self condition) or observing their female partner receiving painful stimulation to her hand (other condition). Prosocial behavior was measured using a monetary economic interaction game with which participants classified as prosocial (N = 12) or selfish (N = 6), depending on whether they cooperated with another player. Empathy-relevant brain activation (anterior insula) was neither enhanced by oxytocin nor positively associated with prosocial behavior. However, oxytocin reduced amygdala activation when participants received painful stimulation themselves (in the nonsocial condition). Surprisingly, this effect was driven by "selfish" participants. The results suggest that selfish individuals may not be as rational and unemotional as usually suggested, their actions being determined by their feeling anxious rather than by reason.

2008 APA, all rights reserved

Figures

Figure 1. Subjective unpleasantness ratings: Average unpleasantness…
Figure 1. Subjective unpleasantness ratings: Average unpleasantness ratings for the conditions self (green) and other (red), placebo (filled), and oxytocin (shaded) with error bars (SEM), * p < .01.
Figure 2. Empathic brain responses: Activation in…
Figure 2. Empathic brain responses: Activation in the right anterior insula (AI) revealed by conjunction analyses depicting shared activation in painful versus nonpainful trials in the self and other placebo condition. Threshold is set at p < .001, uncorrected.
Figure 3. Individual differences in empathic brain…
Figure 3. Individual differences in empathic brain responses: (A) Correlation of parameter estimates in the anterior insula/operculum for (pain vs. no pain) in the placebo other condition and subjective unpleasantness ratings. (B) Activation revealed in anterior insula/operculum for the contrast (pain vs. no pain) in the placebo other condition (red) and for the regression analysis of unpleasantness ratings on the contrast (pain vs. no pain) in the placebo other condition (yellow). Threshold is set at p < .001, uncorrected.
Figure 4. Effects of oxytocin and types…
Figure 4. Effects of oxytocin and types on amygdala: The bar diagram illustrates parameter estimates derived from the right amygdala (21, −3, 18) revealed in the three-way interaction (pain, drug, prosociality) in self. Contrast estimates on the contrast (pain vs. no pain) are shown for the self (green) and the other (red) in the placebo (filled), and oxytocin (shaded) conditions, separately for prosocial and selfish participants. Threshold is set at p < .001, uncorrected.

References

    1. Adolphs R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 169–177.
    1. Ashraf N., Bohnet I., & Piankov N. (2006). Decomposing trust and trustworthiness. Experimental Economics, 9, 193–208.
    1. Avenanti A., Bueti D., Galati G., & Aglioti S. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 955–960.
    1. Avenanti A., Paluello I. M., Bufalari I., & Aglioti S. M. (2006). Stimulus-driven modulation of motor-evoked potentials during observation of others' pain. NeuroImage, 32, 316–324.
    1. Bale T. L., Davis A. M., Auger A. P., Dorsa D. M., & McCarthy M. M. (2001). CNS region-specific oxytocin receptor expression: Importance in regulation of anxiety and sex behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2546–2552.
    1. Batson C. D., & Shaw L. L. (1991). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 107–122.
    1. Baumgartner T., Heinrichs M., Vonlanthen A., Fischbacher U., & Fehr E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron, 58, 639–650.
    1. Berg J., Dickhaut J., & McCabe K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social-history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.
    1. Bishop S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 307–316.
    1. Born J., Lange T., Kern W., McGregor G. P., Bickel U., & Fehm H. L. (2002). Sniffing neuropeptides: A transnasal approach to the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 514–516.
    1. Botvinick M., Jha A. P., Bylsma L. M., Fabian S. A., Solomon P. E., & Prkachin K. M. (2005). Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage, 25, 312–319.
    1. Brosig J., Weimann J., & Yang C. L. (2003). The hot versus cold effect in a simple bargaining experiment. Experimental Economics, 6, 75–90.
    1. Bufalari I., Aprile T., Avenanti A., Di Russo F., & Aglioti S. M. (2007). Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2553–2561.
    1. Campbell A. (2007). Attachment, aggression and affiliation: The role of oxytocin in female social behavior. Biological Psychology.
    1. Carter C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818.
    1. Cheng Y., Lin C. P., Liu H. L., Hsu Y. Y., Lim K. E., Hung D., et al. (2007). Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Current Biology, 17, 1708–1713.
    1. Decety J., & Jackson P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.
    1. Decety J., & Lamm C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163.
    1. de Vignemont F., & Singer T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.
    1. Domes G., Heinrichs M., Glascher J., Buchel C., Braus D. F., & Herpertz S. C. (2007). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 1187–1190.
    1. Domes G., Heinrichs M., Michel A., Berger C., & Herpertz S. C. (2007). Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 731–733.
    1. Eisenberg N., Fabes R. A., Miller P. A., Fultz J., Shell R., Mathy R. M., et al. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 55–66.
    1. Eisenberg N., Miller P. A., Schaller M., Fabes R. A., Fultz J., Shell R., et al. (1989). The role of sympathy and altruistic personality traits in helping: A reexamination. Journal of Personality, 57, 41–67.
    1. Etkin A., & Wager T. D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1476–1488.
    1. Fehr E., & Fischbacher U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785–791.
    1. Ferguson J. N., Aldag J. M., Insel T. R., & Young L. J. (2001). Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 8278–8285.
    1. Fischer H., Tillfors M., Furmark T., & Fredrikson M. (2001). Dispositional pessimism and amygdala activity: A PET study in healthy volunteers. NeuroReport, 12, 1635–1638.
    1. Gallese V. (2003). The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36, 171–180.
    1. Gu X., & Han S. (2007). Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. NeuroImage, 36, 256–267.
    1. Haas B. W., Omura K., Constable R. T., & Canli T. (2007). Emotional conflict and neuroticism: Personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 249–256.
    1. Hein G., & Singer T. (2008). I feel how you feel: Neural correlates of empathy in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 153–158.
    1. Heinrichs M., Baumgartner T., Kirschbaum C., & Ehlert U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1389–1398.
    1. Heinrichs M., & Domes G. (2008). Neuropeptides and social behavior: Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Progress in Brain Research, 170, 337–350.
    1. Heinrichs M., Meinlschmidt G., Wippich W., Ehlert U., & Hellhammer D. H. (2004). Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory. Physiology and Behavior, 83, 31–38.
    1. Huber D., Veinante P., & Stoop R. (2005). Vasopressin and oxytocin excite distinct neuronal populations in the central amygdala. Science, 308(5719), 245–248.
    1. Insel T. R., & Shapiro L. E. (1992). Oxytocin receptors and maternal behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 652, 122–141.
    1. Insel T. R., & Young L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 129–136.
    1. Jabbi M., Swart M., & Keysers C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.
    1. Jackson P. L., Brunet E., Meltzoff A. N., & Decety J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752–761.
    1. Jackson P. L., Meltzoff A. N., & Decety J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage, 24, 771–779.
    1. Keysers C., Wicker B., Gazzola V., Anton J. L., Fogassi L., & Gallese V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron, 42, 335–346.
    1. Kirsch P., Esslinger C., Chen Q., Mier D., Lis S., Siddhanti S., et al. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 11489–11493.
    1. Kosfeld M., Heinrichs M., Zak P. J., Fischbacher U., & Fehr E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.
    1. Lamm C., Batson C. D., & Decety J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.
    1. Landgraf R., & Neumann I. D. (2004). Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: A dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 25, 150–176.
    1. LeDoux J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23, 727–738.
    1. Lim M. M., & Young L. J. (2006). Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative behavior and social bonding in animals. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 506–517.
    1. Moriguchi Y., Decety J., Ohnishi T., Maeda M., Mori T., Nemoto K., et al. (2007). Empathy and judging other's pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2223–2234.
    1. Morrison I., & Downing P. E. (2007). Organization of felt and seen pain responses in anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage, 37, 642–651.
    1. Morrison I., Lloyd D., di Pellegrino G., & Roberts N. (2004). Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: Is empathy a multisensory issue? Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 270–278.
    1. Neumann I. D., Kromer S. A., Toschi N., & Ebner K. (2000). Brain oxytocin inhibits the (re)activity of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in male rats: Involvement of hypothalamic and limbic brain regions. Regulatory Peptides, 96, 31–38.
    1. O'Doherty J. P., Deichmann R., Critchley H. D., & Dolan R. J. (2002). Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron, 33, 815–826.
    1. Parker K. J., Buckmaster C. L., Schatzberg A. F., & Lyons D. M. (2005). Intranasal oxytocin administration attenuates the ACTH stress response in monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 924–929.
    1. Pedersen C. A. (1997). Oxytocin control of maternal behavior. Regulation by sex steroids and offspring stimuli. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807, 126–145.
    1. Penner L. A., Dovidio J. F., Piliavin J. A., & Schroeder D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 365–392.
    1. Petrovic P., Kalisch R., Singer T., & Dolan R. J. (2008). Oxytocin attenuates affective evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 6607–6615.
    1. Peyron R., Laurent B., & Garcia-Larrea L. (2000). Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiologie Clinique, 30, 263–288.
    1. Phelps E. A., O'Connor K. J., Gatenby J. C., Gore J. C., Grillon C., & Davis M. (2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 437–441.
    1. Pitman R. K., Orr S. P., & Lasko N. B. (1993). Effects of intranasal vasopressin and oxytocin on physiologic responding during personal combat imagery in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 48, 107–117.
    1. Preston S. D., & de Waal F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.
    1. Rosen J. B., & Donley M. P. (2006). Animal studies of amygdala function in fear and uncertainty: Relevance to human research. Biological Psychology, 73, 49–60.
    1. Saarela M. V., Hlushchuk Y., Williams A. C. D., Schurmann M., Kalso E., & Hari R. (2007). The compassionate brain: Humans detect intensity of pain from another's face. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 230–237.
    1. Singer T., Seymour B., O'Doherty J., Kaube H., Dolan R. J., & Frith C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(5661), 1157–1162.
    1. Singer T., Seymour B., O'Doherty J., Stephan K. E., Dolan R. J., & Frith C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469.
    1. Spielberger C. D., Gorsuch R. L., Lushene R., Vagg P. R., & Jacobs G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists.
    1. Wicker B., Keysers C., Plailly J., Royet J. P., Gallese V., & Rizzolatti G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.
    1. Young L. J., & Wang Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1048–1054.
    1. Zak P. J., Stanton A. A., & Ahmadi S. (2007). Oxytocin increases generosity in humans. PLoS ONE, 2, 1–5.
    1. Zald D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41(1), 88–123.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe