Does the management of personal integrity information lead to differing participation rates and response patterns in mental health surveys with young adults? A three-armed methodological experiment

Claes Andersson, Marcus Bendtsen, Petra Lindfors, Olof Molander, Philip Lindner, Naira Topooco, Karin Engström, Anne H Berman, Claes Andersson, Marcus Bendtsen, Petra Lindfors, Olof Molander, Philip Lindner, Naira Topooco, Karin Engström, Anne H Berman

Abstract

Objectives: This study evaluates whether initiation rates, completion rates, response patterns and prevalence of psychiatric conditions differ by level of personal integrity information given to prospective participants in an online mental health self-report survey.

Methods: A three-arm, parallel-group, single-blind experiment was conducted among students from two Swedish universities. Consenting participants following e-mail invitation answered the World Health Organization (WHO) World Mental Health-International College Student (WMH-ICS) mental health self-report survey, screening for eight psychiatric conditions. Random allocation meant consenting to respond (1) anonymously; (2) confidentially, or (3) confidentially, where the respondent also gave consent for collection of register data.

Results: No evidence was found for overall between-group differences with respect to (1) pressing a hyperlink to the survey in the invitation email; and (2) abandoning the questionnaire before completion. However, participation consent and self-reported depression were in the direction of higher levels for the anonymous group compared to the two confidential groups.

Conclusions: Consent to participate is marginally affected by different levels of personal integrity information. Current standard participant information procedures may not engage participants to read the information thoroughly, and online self-report mental health surveys may reduce stigma and thus be less subject to social desirability bias.

Keywords: anonymous; confidential; mental health; online survey; personal integrity; register data.

Conflict of interest statement

No conflicts of interest are declared.

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Group‐specific information provided in invitation email and study information
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Four levels of group comparisons
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Participant flow. a = Percent of those who received email. b = Percent of those who proceeded to study information. c = Percent of those who provided consent. Italicized numbers indicate participants who did not proceed to the next step, thereby abandoning the survey

References

    1. Albaum, G. (1987). Do source and anonymity affect mail survey results? Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 15, 74–81. 10.1007/BF02722173
    1. Andersson, C. , Bendtsen, M. , Lindfors, P. , Engström, K. , Lindner, P. , Topooco, N. , & Berman, A. H. (2020). Response patterns by personal integrity.
    1. Auerbach, R. P. , Mortier, P. , Bruffaerts, R. , Alonso, J. , Benjet, C. , Cuijpers, P. , Demyttenaere, K. , Ebert, D. D. , Green, J. G. , Hasking, P. , Murray, E. , Nock, M. K. , Pinder‐Amaker, S. , Sampson, N. A. , Stein, D. J. , Vilagut, G. , Zaslavsky, A. M. , Kessler, R. C. , Kessler R. C., & WHO WMH‐ICS Collaborators . (2018). WHO World mental health surveys international College student project: Prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(7), 623–638. 10.1037/abn0000362
    1. Beatty, J. R. , Chase, S. K. , & Ondersma, S. J. (2014). A randomized study of the effect of anonymity, quasi‐anonymity, and Certificates of Confidentiality on postpartum women’s disclosure of sensitive information. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 134, 280–284. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.10.016
    1. Beebe, T. J. , Harrison, P. A. , Park, E. , McRae, J. A. , & Evans, J. (2006). The effects of data collection mode and disclosure on adolescent reporting of health behavior. Social Science Computer Review, 24(4), 476–488. 10.1177/0894439306288690
    1. Bjarnason, T. , & Adalbjarnardottir, S. (2000). Anonymity and confidentiality in school surveys on alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis use. Journal of Drug Issues, 30(2), 335–343. 10.1177/002204260003000206
    1. Campbell, M. J. , & Waters, W. E. (1990). Does anonymity increase response rate in postal questionnaire surveys about sensitive subjects? A randomised trial. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 44(1), 75–76. 10.1136/jech.44.1.75
    1. Chase, S. K. , Beatty, J. R. , & Ondersma, S. J. (2013). A randomized trial of the effects of anonymity and quasi anonymity on disclosure of child maltreatment‐related outcomes among postpartum women. Child Maltreatment, 16(1), 33–40. 10.1177/1077559510387659
    1. Cuijpers, P. , Auerbach, R. P. , Benjet, C. , Bruffaerts, R. , Ebert, D. , Karyotaki, E. , & Kessler, R. C. (2019). The World Health Organization World Mental Health International College student initiative: An overview. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 28(2), e1761. 10.1002/mpr.1761
    1. Da Silva, M. , Coeli, C. , Ventura, M. , Palacios, M. , Magnanini, M. , Camargo, T. , & Camargo, K. (2012). Informed consent for record linkage: A systematic review. Journal of Medical Ethics, 38(10), 639‐642.
    1. D’Orazio, M. (2015). Integration and imputation of survey data in R: The StatMatch package. Revista Romana de Statistica, 63(2), 57–68.
    1. Douglas, B. D. , McGorray, E. L. , & Ewell, P. J. (2021). Some researchers wear yellow pants, but even fewer participants read consent forms: Exploring and improving consent form reading in human subjects research. Psychological Methods, 26(1), 61–68. 10.1037/met0000267
    1. Durant, L. E. , Carey, M. P. , & Schroder, K. E. E. (2002). Effects of anonymity, gender, and erotophilia on the quality of data obtained from self‐reports of socially sensitive behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(5), 439–467. 10.1023/A:1020419023766
    1. Esposito, J. L. , Agard, E. , & Rosnow, R. L. (1984). Can confidentiality of data pay off? Personality and Individual Differences, 5(4), 477‐480. 10.1016/0191-8869(84)90016-3
    1. Fear, N. T. , Seddon, R. , Jones, N. , Greenberg, N. , & Wessley, S. (2012). Does anonymity increase the reporting om mental health symptoms? BMC Public Health, 12, 797. 10.1186/1471-2458-12-797
    1. Futrell, C. M. , Stem, D. E. , & Fortune, B. D. (1978). Effects of signed versus unsigned internally administered questionnaires for managers, Journal of Business Research, 6(2), 91‐98. 10.1016/0148-2963(78)90001-2
    1. Hendra, R. , & Hill, A. (2018). Rethinking response rates: New evidence of little relationship between survey response rates and nonresponse bias. Evaluation Review, 43(5), 307‐330. 10.1177/0193841x18807719
    1. Krumpal, I. (2013). Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: A literature review. Quality and Quantity, 47(4), 2025–2047. 10.1007/s11135-011-9640-9
    1. Kundig, F. , Staines, A. , Kinge, T. , & Perneger, T. V. (2011). Numbering questionnaires had no impact on the response rate and only a slight influence on the response content of a patient safety culture survey: A randomized trial. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 64(11), 1262‐1265. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.02.008
    1. Malvin, J. H. & Moskowitz, J. M. (1983). Anonymous versus identifiable self‐reports of adolescent drug attitudes, intentions, and use. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(4), 557.
    1. McKee, D. O. (1992). The effect of using questionnaire identification code and message about non‐response follow‐up plans on mail survey response characteristics. Market Research Society, 23(2), 179‐191.
    1. Murdoch, M. , Simon, A. B. , Pousny, M. A. , Bangerter, A. K. , Grill, J. P. , Noorbaloochi, S. , & Partin, M. R. (2014). Impact of different privacy conditions and incentives on survey response rate, participant representativeness, and disclosure of sensitive information: A randomized controlled trial. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 14(1), 90‐100. 10.1186/1471-2288-14-90
    1. Nederhof, A. J. (1985). Methods of coping with social desirability bias: A review. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15(3), 263–280. 10.1002/ejsp.2420150303
    1. Olson, C. B. , Stander, V. A. , & Merrill, L. L. (2004). The influence of survey confidentiality and construct measurement in estimating rates of childhood victimization among Navy recruits, Military Psychology, 16(1), 53–69. 10.1207/s15327876mp1601_4
    1. O’Malley, P. M. , Johnston, L. D. , Bachman, J. G. , & Schulenberg, J. (2000). A comparison of confidential versus anonymous survey procedures: Effects on reporting of drug use and related attitudes and beliefs in a national study of students. Journal of Drug Issues, 30(1), 35–54. 10.1177/002204260003000103
    1. Ong, A. D. , & Weiss, D. J. (2000). The impact of anonymity on responses to sensitive questions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1691–1708. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02462.x
    1. Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17(11), 776–783. 10.1037/h0043424
    1. Pedersen, E. , Neighbors, C. , Tidwell, J. , & Lostutter, T. (2011). Do undergraduate student research participants read psychological research consent forms? Examining memory effects, condition effects, and individual differences. Ethics & Behavior, 21(4), 332–350. 10.1080/10508422.2011.585601
    1. Richman, W. L. , Weisband, S. , Kiesler, S. , & Drasgow, F. (1999). A meta‐analytic study of social disability distortion in computer‐administered questionnaires, traditional questionnaires, and interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 754‐775. 10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.754
    1. Rolnick, S. J. , Gross, C. R. , Garrard, J. , & Gibson, R. W. (1989). A comparison of response rate, data quality, and cost in the collection of data on sexual history and personal behaviors: Mail survey approaches and in‐person interview. American Journal of Epidemiology, 129(5), 1052–1061. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115209
    1. Singer, E. , Mathiowetz, N. A. , & Couper, M. P. (1993). The impact of privacy and confidentiality concerns on survey participation: The case of the 1990 U.S. Census. Public Opinion Quarterly, 57(4), 465–482.
    1. Slegers, C. , Zion, D. , Glass, D. , Kelsall, H. , Fritschi, L. , Brown, N. , & Loff, B. (2015). Why do people participate in epidemiological research? Journal of bioethical inquiry, 12(2), 227–237. 10.1007/s11673-015-9611-2
    1. Stander, V. A. , Olson, C. B. , & Merrill, L. L. (2002). Self‐definition as a survivor of childhood sexual abuse among navy recruits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 369–377.
    1. van de Looij‐Jansen, P. M. , Goldschmeding, J. E. J. , & de Wilde, E. J. (2006). Comparison of anonymous versus confidential survey procedures: Effects on health indicators in Dutch adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(4), 652–658. 10.1007/s10964-005-9027-0
    1. Werch, C. E. (1990). Two procedures to reduce response bias in reports of alcohol consumption. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51(4), 327–330.
    1. World Medical Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Journal of the American Medical Association, 310(20), 2191–2194. 10.1001/jama.2013.281053
    1. Zagumny, M. J. , Ramsey, R. J. , & Upchurch, M. P. (1996). Is anonymity important in AIDS survey research? Psychological Reports, 78(1), 270. 10.2466/pr0.1996.78.1.270

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe