Continuous theta burst stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases medium load working memory performance in healthy humans

Nathalie Schicktanz, Matthias Fastenrath, Annette Milnik, Klara Spalek, Bianca Auschra, Thomas Nyffeler, Andreas Papassotiropoulos, Dominique J-F de Quervain, Kyrill Schwegler, Nathalie Schicktanz, Matthias Fastenrath, Annette Milnik, Klara Spalek, Bianca Auschra, Thomas Nyffeler, Andreas Papassotiropoulos, Dominique J-F de Quervain, Kyrill Schwegler

Abstract

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) plays a key role in working memory. Evidence indicates that transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the DLPFC can interfere with working memory performance. Here we investigated for how long continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) over the DLPFC decreases working memory performance and whether the effect of cTBS on performance depends on working memory load. Forty healthy young subjects received either cTBS over the left DLPFC or sham stimulation before performing a 2-, and 3-back working memory letter task. An additional 0-back condition served as a non-memory-related control, measuring general attention. cTBS over the left DLPFC significantly impaired 2-back working memory performance for about 15 min, whereas 3-back and 0-back performances were not significantly affected. Our results indicate that the effect of left DLPFC cTBS on working memory performance lasts for roughly 15 min and depends on working memory load.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Brain activity related to working…
Fig 1. Brain activity related to working memory.
Prefrontal activity, color-coded t values (pwhole-brain-FWE-corrected< 0.05; N = 707). The white circles depict the area within the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex stimulated by TMS (centered at [–53, 31, 25] in Montreal Neurological Institute space). Activations are overlaid on coronal (top left), sagittal (top right), and axial (bottom left) sections of a study-specific group template. L, left side of the brain; R, right side of the brain.
Fig 2. Stimulation effects on delta ACC…
Fig 2. Stimulation effects on delta ACC 2-back, separately for each block after TMS.
A nominal significant effect of stimulation could be detected up to block four, corresponding to 15 min after TMS (including 3 min for participant reallocation to the computer screen after TMS). Values higher than 100 indicate an improved performance compared to the last block before TMS, while values below 100 indicate decreased performance.

References

    1. Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4: 417–23.
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS. Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93: 13473–80.
    1. Postle BR. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience. 2006;139: 23–38.
    1. D'Esposito M. From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;362: 761–72.
    1. Sandrini M, Umilta C, Rusconi E. The use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a new synthesis of methodological issues. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35: 516–36. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.005
    1. Silvanto J, Muggleton N, Walsh V. State-dependency in brain stimulation studies of perception and cognition. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2008;12: 447–54. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.004
    1. Fitzgerald PB, Fountain S, Daskalakis ZJ. A comprehensive review of the effects of rTMS on motor cortical excitability and inhibition. Clinical neurophysiology: official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2006;117: 2584–96.
    1. Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45: 201–6.
    1. Robertson EM, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Studies in cognition: the problems solved and created by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15: 948–60.
    1. Nyffeler T, Wurtz P, Lüscher HR, Hess CW, Senn W, Pflugshaupt T, et al. Repetitive TMS over the human oculomotor cortex: comparison of 1-Hz and theta burst stimulation. Neuroscience letters. 2006;409: 57–60.
    1. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25: 46–59.
    1. Mottaghy F, Gangitano M, Sparing R, Krause B, Pascual-Leone A. Segregation of areas related to visual working memory in the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12: 369–75.
    1. Morgan HM, Jackson MC, van Koningsbruggen MG, Shapiro KL, Linden DE. Frontal and parietal theta burst TMS impairs working memory for visual-spatial conjunctions. Brain stimulation. 2013;6: 122–9. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.001
    1. Lee TG, D'Esposito M. The dynamic nature of top-down signals originating from prefrontal cortex: a combined fMRI-TMS study. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32: 15458–66. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0627-12.2012
    1. Kaminski JA, Korb FM, Villringer A, Ott DV. Transcranial magnetic stimulation intensities in cognitive paradigms. PLoS One. 2011;6: e24836 10.1371/journal.pone.0024836
    1. Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C, Clemente IC, Molinuevo JL, Bargallo N, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on brain function and cognition among elders with memory dysfunction. A randomized sham-controlled study. Cerebral cortex. 2006;16: 1487–93.
    1. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9: 97–113.
    1. Heck A, Fastenrath M, Ackermann S, Auschra B, Bickel H, Coynel D, et al. Converging genetic and functional brain imaging evidence links neuronal excitability to working memory, psychiatric disease, and brain activity. Neuron. 2014;81: 1203–13. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.010
    1. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, et al. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human brain mapping. 2000;10: 120–31.
    1. Schicktanz N, Schwegler K, Fastenrath M, Spalek K, Milnik A, Papassotiropoulos A, et al. Motor threshold predicts working memory performance in healthy humans. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology. 2014;1: 69–73. 10.1002/acn3.22
    1. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD. Das State-Trait-Angstinventar Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. Weinheim: Beltz; 1981.
    1. Gevins A, Cutillo B. Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;87: 128–43.
    1. Stanislaw H, Todorov N. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1999;31: 137–49.
    1. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage. 2012;60: 830–46. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050
    1. Manelis A, Reder LM. He who is well prepared has half won the battle: An FMRI study of task preparation. Cerebral cortex. 2015;25: 726–35. 10.1093/cercor/bht262
    1. Honey GD, Fu CH, Kim J, Brammer MJ, Croudace TJ, Suckling J, et al. Effects of verbal working memory load on corticocortical connectivity modeled by path analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. NeuroImage. 2002;17: 573–82.
    1. Ma L, Steinberg JL, Hasan KM, Narayana PA, Kramer LA, Moeller FG. Working memory load modulation of parieto-frontal connections: evidence from dynamic causal modeling. Human brain mapping. 2012;33: 1850–67. 10.1002/hbm.21329
    1. Jonides J, Schumacher EH, Smith EE, Lauber EJ, Awh E, Minoshima S, et al. Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1997;9: 462–75. 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.462
    1. Gamboa OL, Antal A, Moliadze V, Paulus W. Simply longer is not better: reversal of theta burst after-effect with prolonged stimulation. Experimental brain research. 2010;204: 181–7. 10.1007/s00221-010-2293-4
    1. Nyffeler T, Wurtz P, Lscher H, Hess C, Senn W, Pflugshaupt T, et al. Extending lifetime of plastic changes in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24: 2961–6.
    1. Goldsworthy MR, Pitcher JB, Ridding MC. Neuroplastic modulation of inhibitory motor cortical networks by spaced theta burst stimulation protocols. Brain stimulation. 2013;6: 340–5. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.06.005
    1. Goldsworthy MR, Muller-Dahlhaus F, Ridding MC, Ziemann U. Resistant Against De-depression: LTD-Like Plasticity in the Human Motor Cortex Induced by Spaced cTBS. Cerebral cortex. 2014. January 31 10.1093/cercor/bht353
    1. Barch DM. The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1: 321–53.
    1. Balanza-Martinez V, Rubio C, Selva-Vera G, Martinez-Aran A, Sanchez-Moreno J, Salazar-Fraile J, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of relatives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32: 1426–38. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.019
    1. Doyle AE. Executive functions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67 Suppl 8: 21–6.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren