Video game telemetry as a critical tool in the study of complex skill learning

Joseph J Thompson, Mark R Blair, Lihan Chen, Andrew J Henrey, Joseph J Thompson, Mark R Blair, Lihan Chen, Andrew J Henrey

Abstract

Cognitive science has long shown interest in expertise, in part because prediction and control of expert development would have immense practical value. Most studies in this area investigate expertise by comparing experts with novices. The reliance on contrastive samples in studies of human expertise only yields deep insight into development where differences are important throughout skill acquisition. This reliance may be pernicious where the predictive importance of variables is not constant across levels of expertise. Before the development of sophisticated machine learning tools for data mining larger samples, and indeed, before such samples were available, it was difficult to test the implicit assumption of static variable importance in expertise development. To investigate if this reliance may have imposed critical restrictions on the understanding of complex skill development, we adopted an alternative method, the online acquisition of telemetry data from a common daily activity for many: video gaming. Using measures of cognitive-motor, attentional, and perceptual processing extracted from game data from 3360 Real-Time Strategy players at 7 different levels of expertise, we identified 12 variables relevant to expertise. We show that the static variable importance assumption is false--the predictive importance of these variables shifted as the levels of expertise increased--and, at least in our dataset, that a contrastive approach would have been misleading. The finding that variable importance is not static across levels of expertise suggests that large, diverse datasets of sustained cognitive-motor performance are crucial for an understanding of expertise in real-world contexts. We also identify plausible cognitive markers of expertise.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Variable Importance.
Figure 1. Variable Importance.
The rank of the permutation importance for each variable. Each column refers to a random forrest classification. Grid colors and numbers reflect ranks. White numbers indicate that the variable is above the cuttoff defined by the control variable (see Supplementary Materials and methods in Materials S1).
Figure 2. Bronze-Gold Permutation Importance.
Figure 2. Bronze-Gold Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Bronze-Gold classifier. Classification rate: 82.32%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 77.81%.
Figure 3. Silver-Platinum Permutation Importance.
Figure 3. Silver-Platinum Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Silver-Platinum classifier. Classification rate: 78.27%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 70.03%.
Figure 4. Gold-Diamond Permutation Importance.
Figure 4. Gold-Diamond Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Gold-Diamond classifier. Classification rate: 79.01%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 59.31%.
Figure 5. Platinum-Masters Permutation Importance.
Figure 5. Platinum-Masters Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Platinum-Master classifier. Classification rate: 80.70%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 56.63%.
Figure 6. Diamond-Professional Permutation Importance.
Figure 6. Diamond-Professional Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Diamond-Professional classifier. Classification rate: 96.75%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 93.61%.
Figure 7. Bronze-Professional Permutation Importance.
Figure 7. Bronze-Professional Permutation Importance.
Histograms for permuted importance values from 25 conditional inference forests for each of the 16 variables used in the Bronze-Professional classifier. Classification rate: 98.59%. Baseline accuracy of classifier from choosing more common class: 75.23%.
Figure 8. Perception Action Cycles (PACs).
Figure 8. Perception Action Cycles (PACs).
Actions and attention shifts for a typical StarCraft 2 player over 15 seconds. Each vertical line tic represents a single action. Notice that most aspects of the PAC become faster with an increase in League.

References

    1. Proctor RW, Vu KP (2006) Laboratory studies of training, skill acquisition, and retention of performance. In the Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance, K.A. Ericsson, Charness N, Feltovich PJ, Hoffman RR Eds. Cambridge, MA: CUP. 265–286.
    1. Logan GD (1988) Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol. Rev. 95 (4): 492–527.
    1. Palmeri TJ (1997) Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. JEP:LMC 23 (2): 324–354.
    1. Fleishman EA, Parker JF (1960) Ability Factors and Component Performance Measures as Predictors of Complex Tracking Behavior. Psychol. Monogr. 74 (16): 1–36.
    1. Charness N, Tuffiash M, Krampe R, Reingold E, Vasyukova E (2005) The Role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 151–165.
    1. Ericsson KA, Williams AM (2007) Capturing naturally occurring superior performance in the laboratory: Translational research on expert performance. JEP:A 13(3): 115–123.
    1. Abreu AM, Macaluso E, Azevedo RT, Cesari P, Urgesi C, et al. (2012) Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35: 1646–1654.
    1. Reingold EM, Charness N, Pomplun M, Stampe DM (2001) Visual span in expert chess players: Evidence from eye movements. Psychol. Sci. 12(1): 48–55.
    1. Chase WG, Simon HA (1973) Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychol. 4: 55–81.
    1. Schmidt HG, Boshuizen HP (1993) On the origin of intermediate effects in clinical case recall. Mem Cognition. 21: 338–351.
    1. Reitman JS (1976) Skilled perception in Go: deducing memory structures from inter-response times. Cognitive Psychol. 8: 336–356.
    1. de Groot AD (2008) Thought and choice in chess, AUP.
    1. Charness N (1983) Age, skill, and bridge bidding: A chronometric analysis. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Beh. 22(4): 406–416.
    1. Ericsson KA, Charness N (1994) Expert performance. Am Psychol. 49(8): 725–747.
    1. Chessgames Services LLC (2001–2012) : online chess database and Community: Statistics page. Available: . Accessed 2012 Jul 9.
    1. Ericsson KA (2006) An introduction to “the Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance: Its development, organization, and content”. K.A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, R. R. Hoffman, Eds. Cambridge, MA: CUP, 3–20.
    1. Sutter JD (2012) Wired for success or destruction? Available: . Accessed 2012 Aug 24.
    1. Blizzard Entertainment (2011) Master League. Available: post) . Accessed 2013 Jan 21.
    1. Pashler H, Johnston JC (1989) Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Q J Exp Psychol-A. 41(1): 19–45.
    1. Schneider W, Schiffrin RM (1977) Controlled and automatic human Information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psyc Rev. 84(1): 1–66.
    1. Schiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psyc Rev. 84(2): 127–188.
    1. Sellers BC, Fincannon T, Jentsch F (2012) The effects of autonomy and cognitive abilities on workload and supervisory control of unmanned systems. Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society 56th annual meeting 56: 1039–1043.
    1. Salvucci DD, Goldberg JH (2000) Identifying Fixations and Saccades in Eye-Tracking Protocols. In Proceedings of the eye tracking research and applications symposium. New York: ACM Press: 71–88.
    1. Land MF, Hayhoe M (2001) In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vision Res. 41: 3559–3565.
    1. Breiman L (1984) Classification and regression trees. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC. ISBN 0–412–04841–8.
    1. Breiman L (1996) Bagging Predictors. Mach. Learning 24: 123–140.
    1. Breiman L (2001) Random Forests. Mach. Learning 45: 5–32.
    1. Strobl C, Malley J, Tutz G (2009) An introduction to recursive partitioning: rationale, application and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging and random forests. Psychol Methods. 14(4): 323–348.
    1. Strobl C, Boulesteix A, Zeileis A, Hothorn T (2006) Bias in random forest variable importance measures: Illustrations, sources and a solution. BMC Bioinformatics 8(25) doi:10.1186/1471-2105-8-25.
    1. Linkletter C, Bingham D, Hengartner N, Higdon D, Ye KQ (2006) Variable selection for gaussian process models in computer experiments. Technometrics 48(4): 478–490.
    1. Ericcson AK, Towne TJ (2010) Expertise. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 1(3): 404–416.
    1. Mané A, Donchin E (1989) The Space Fortress game. Acta Psychol. 71: 17–22.
    1. Stern Y, Blumen HM, Rich LW, Richards A, Herzberg G, et al. (2011) Space Fortress game training and executive control in older adults: A pilot intervention. Aging Neuropsycholol C 18(6): 653–677.
    1. Lewis JM, Trinh P, Kirsh D (2011) A corpus analysis of strategy video game play in StarCraft: Brood War. In expanding the space of cognitive science: Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the cognitive science society. L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, T. F. Shipley, Eds. Austin, TX. 687–692.
    1. Simon HA, Chase WG (1973) Skill in chess: Experiments with chess-playing tasks and computer simulation of skilled performance throw light on some human perceptual and memory processes. AmSci. 61(4): 394–403.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren