A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity

Monica D Rosenberg, Emily S Finn, Dustin Scheinost, Xenophon Papademetris, Xilin Shen, R Todd Constable, Marvin M Chun, Monica D Rosenberg, Emily S Finn, Dustin Scheinost, Xenophon Papademetris, Xilin Shen, R Todd Constable, Marvin M Chun

Abstract

Although attention plays a ubiquitous role in perception and cognition, researchers lack a simple way to measure a person's overall attentional abilities. Because behavioral measures are diverse and difficult to standardize, we pursued a neuromarker of an important aspect of attention, sustained attention, using functional magnetic resonance imaging. To this end, we identified functional brain networks whose strength during a sustained attention task predicted individual differences in performance. Models based on these networks generalized to previously unseen individuals, even predicting performance from resting-state connectivity alone. Furthermore, these same models predicted a clinical measure of attention--symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder--from resting-state connectivity in an independent sample of children and adolescents. These results demonstrate that whole-brain functional network strength provides a broadly applicable neuromarker of sustained attention.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Functional connectivity models predict sustained attention performance. Scatter plots show correlations between observed gradCPT d′ values and predictions by positive and negative networks and general linear models (GLM) that take into account positive and negative network strength. Network models were iteratively trained on task data from n − 1 subjects in the gradCPT data set and tested on task data (top row) and resting-state data (bottom row) from the left-out individual.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Functional connections predicting gradCPT performance and ADHD-RS scores. (A) The 757 edges in the high-attention network (predicting higher d′ values in the gradCPT sample and lower ADHD-RS scores in the ADHD-200 sample) are visualized in orange. The 630 edges in the low-attention network (predicting lower d′ values in the gradCPT sample and higher ADHD-RS scores in the ADHD-200 sample) are visualized in blue. Edges that appear in both the gradCPT and ADHD networks appear in bold. Macroscale regions include prefrontal cortex (PFC), motor cortex (Mot), insula (Ins), parietal (Par), temporal (Tem), occipital (Occ), limbic (including the cingulate cortex, amygdala and hippocampus; Lim), cerebellum (Cer), subcortical (thalamus and striatum; Sub), brainstem (Bsm). (B) Differences in the number of edges between each pair of macroscale regions, calculated by subtracting the number of edges in the low-attention network from the number in the high-attention network. (C) Differences in the number of edges between each pair of canonical networks, calculated by subtracting the number of edges in the low-attention network from the number in the high-attention network. Canonical networks include the subcortical-cerebellum (SubC), motor (MT), medial frontal (MF), visual I (VI), visual II (VII), visual association (VA), default mode (DM), and frontoparietal (FP).
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Sustained Attention Network (SAN) models, defined with gradCPT subjects, significantly predict scores on the ADHD-Rating Scale (ADHD-RS) in an independent sample of children and adolescents from the ADHD-200 dataset. Predictions are negatively correlated with ADHD-RS scores because models were trained to predict d′; thus, higher predictions correspond to better attentional abilities and lower ADHD-RS scores. These individuals were diagnosed with ADHD (solid dots) or as typically developing controls (TDC, hollow dots).
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Connectivity models defined on ADHD-200 data predict gradCPT performance in an independent group of participants. Scatter plots show predictions of models defined using edges negatively (orange) and positively (blue) related to ADHD-RS scores in ADHD-200 resting state data. Predictions of a GLM, which incorporates low- and high-ADHD network strength, are shown in black. These models were applied to gradCPT task (top) and resting-state data (bottom).

References

    1. Cattell RB. Intelligence: its structure, growth and action. Advances in psychology. 1987;35
    1. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105.
    1. Unsworth N, Fukuda K, Awh E, Vogel EK. Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cogn Psychol. 2014;71:1–26.
    1. Kyllonen PC, Christal RE. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence. 1990;14:389–433.
    1. Engle RW, Kane MJ, Tuholski SW. Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. 1999:102–134. doi: 10.1037/a0021324.
    1. Luck SJ, Vogel EK. Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:391–400.
    1. Chun MM, Golomb JD, Turk-Browne NB. A Taxonomy of External and Internal Attention. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010;62:73–101.
    1. Rosenberg MD, Finn ES, Todd Constable R, Chun MM. Predicting moment-to-moment attentional state. Neuroimage. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.032.
    1. Warm JS, Parasuraman R, Matthews G. Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum Factors. 2008;50:433–441.
    1. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222.
    1. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:1263–1276.
    1. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215.
    1. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on Attention Networks as a Model for the Integration of Psychological Science. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;58:1–23.
    1. deBettencourt MT, Cohen JD, Lee RF, Norman KA, Turk-Browne NB. Closed-loop training of attention with real-time brain imaging. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:470–475.
    1. Rosvold HE, Mirsky AF, Sarason I, Bransome ED, Beck LH. A continuous performance test of brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1956;20:343–350.
    1. Riccio C, Reynolds C, Lowe P. Clinical applications of continuous performance tests: Measuring attention and impulsive responding in children and adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;20:559–560.
    1. Esterman M, Noonan SK, Rosenberg M, Degutis J. In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:2712–2723.
    1. Rosenberg M, Noonan S, DeGutis J, Esterman M. Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: a novel gradual-onset continuous performance task. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2013;75:426–439.
    1. Fortenbaugh FC, et al. Sustained Attention Across the Life Span in a Sample of 10,000 Dissociating Ability and Strategy. Psychol Sci. 2015 0956797615594896.
    1. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:65–94.
    1. Shen X, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Graph-theory based parcellation of functional subunits in the brain from resting-state fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1027–1035.
    1. Shen X, Tokoglu F, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. Neuroimage. 2013;82:403–415.
    1. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1059–1069.
    1. Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin. 1980;87:245–251.
    1. Consortium T A.-200. The ADHD-200 Consortium. A Model to Advance the Translational Potential of Neuroimaging in Clinical Neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci. 2012;6:62.
    1. DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, Reid R. ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. Guilford Press; New York: 1998. p. 25.
    1. Dan L, Yu J, Vandenberg SG, Yuemei Z, CAIHONG T. Report on Shanghai norms for the Chinese translation of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. Psychol Rep. 1990;67:531–541.
    1. Finn ES, et al. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1664–1671.
    1. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum. 2012;11:352–65.
    1. Buckner RL. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron. 2013;80:807–815.
    1. Castellanos FX, Proal E. Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: Beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:17–26.
    1. Krain AL, Castellanos FX. Brain development and ADHD. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:433–444.
    1. Huang L, Mo L, Li Y. Measuring the interrelations among multiple paradigms of visual attention: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2012;38:414–428.
    1. Baldassarre A, et al. From the Cover: Individual variability in functional connectivity predicts performance of a perceptual task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:3516–3521.
    1. Smith SM, et al. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:666–682.
    1. Gabrieli JDE, Ghosh SS, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Prediction as a Humanitarian and Pragmatic Contribution from Human Cognitive Neuroscience. Neuron. 2015;85:11–26.
    1. Whelan R, et al. Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature. 2014;512:185–189.
    1. Rosenberg MD, Finn ES, Constable RT, Chun MM. Predicting moment-to-moment attentional state
    1. Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:870–900.
    1. Turk-Browne NB. Functional Interactions as Big Data in the Human Brain. Science (80- ) 2013;342:580–584.
    1. Cao Q, et al. Abnormal neural activity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport. 2006;17:1033–1036.
    1. Tian L, et al. Altered resting-state functional connectivity patterns of anterior cingulate cortex in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2006;400:39–43.
    1. Uddin LQ, et al. Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;169:249–54.
    1. Wang L, et al. Altered small-world brain functional networks in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:638–649.
    1. Fair DA, et al. Atypical default network connectivity in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:1084–91.
    1. Qiu M, et al. Changes of Brain Structure and Function in ADHD Children. Brain Topogr. 2011;24:243–252.
    1. Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Abnormal functional connectivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:443–50.
    1. Cocchi L, et al. Altered Functional Brain Connectivity in a Non-Clinical Sample of Young Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17753–17761.
    1. Joshi A, et al. Unified Framework for Development, Deployment and Robust Testing of Neuroimaging Algorithms. Neuroinformatics. 2011;9:69–84.
    1. Kaufman J, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988.
    1. Friedman L, Glover GH The FBIRN Consortium. Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: Controlling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio (SFNR) differences. Neuroimage. 2006;33:471–481.
    1. Scheinost D, Papademetris X, Constable RT. The impact of image smoothness on intrinsic functional connectivity and head motion confounds. Neuroimage. 2014;95:13–21.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren