The role of education and intellectual activity on cognition

Jeanine M Parisi, George W Rebok, Qian-Li Xue, Linda P Fried, Teresa E Seeman, Elizabeth K Tanner, Tara L Gruenewald, Kevin D Frick, Michelle C Carlson, Jeanine M Parisi, George W Rebok, Qian-Li Xue, Linda P Fried, Teresa E Seeman, Elizabeth K Tanner, Tara L Gruenewald, Kevin D Frick, Michelle C Carlson

Abstract

Although educational attainment has been consistently related to cognition in adulthood, the mechanisms are still unclear. Early education, and other social learning experiences, may provide the skills, knowledge, and interest to pursue intellectual challenges across the life course. Therefore, cognition in adulthood might reflect continued engagement with cognitively complex environments. Using baseline data from the Baltimore Experience Corps Trial, multiple mediation models were applied to examine the combined and unique contributions of intellectual, social, physical, creative, and passive lifestyle activities on the relationship between education and cognition. Separate models were tested for each cognitive outcome (i.e., reading ability, processing speed, memory). With the exception of memory tasks, findings suggest that education-cognition relations are partially explained by frequent participation in intellectual activities. The association between education and cognition was not completely eliminated, however, suggesting that other factors may drive these associations.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Example of multiple mediator model.

References

    1. Albert MS, Jones K, Savage CR, et al. Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychology and Aging. 1995;10(4):578–589.
    1. Lyketsos CG, Chen LS, Anthony JC. Cognitive decline in adulthood: an 11.5-year follow-up of the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(1):58–65.
    1. Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2005.
    1. Tucker-Drob EM, Johnson KE, Jones RN. The cognitive reserve hypothesis: a longitudinal examination of age-associated declines in reasoning and processing speed. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(2):431–446.
    1. Wilson RS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Barnes LL, De Leon CFM, Evans DA. Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology. 2009;72(5):460–465.
    1. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909–1915.
    1. Carlson MC, Brandt J, Carson KA, Kawas CH. Lack of relation between race and cognitive test performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1998;50(5):1499–1501.
    1. Sharp ES, Gatz M. Relationship between education and dementia: an updated systematic review. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2011;25:289–304.
    1. Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Ming Xin Tang, Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1994;271(13):1004–1010.
    1. Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging. 1999;14(2):245–263.
    1. Strain LA, Grabusic CC, Searle MS, Dunn NJ. Continuing and ceasing leisure activities in later life: a longitudinal study. Gerontologist. 2002;42(2):217–223.
    1. Aartsen MJ, Smits CHM, Van Tilburg T, Knipscheer KCPM, Deeg DJH. Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. Journals of Gerontology. 2002;57(2):P153–P162.
    1. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4–10.
    1. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Beckett LA, et al. Cognitive activity in older persons from a geographically defined population. Journals of Gerontology. 1999;54(3):P155–P160.
    1. Bielak AAM. How can we not ’lose it’ if we still don’t understand how to ’use it’? unanswered questions about the influence of activity participation on cognitive performance in older age—a mini-review. Gerontology. 2010;56(5):507–519.
    1. Carlson MC, Parisi JM, Xia J, et al. Lifestyle activities and aging: variety may be the spice of life. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;18:1–9.
    1. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Supplement. 2008;9(1):1–65.
    1. Stine-Morrow EAL, Parisi JM, Morrow DG, Park DC. The effects of an engaged lifestyle on cognitive vitality: a field experiment. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23(4):778–786.
    1. Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Krueger KR, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11(4):400–407.
    1. Lövdén M, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U, Schaefer S, Schmiedek F. A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136(4):659–676.
    1. Schooler C, Mulatu MS. The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16(3):466–482.
    1. Bielak AAM, Hughes TF, Small BJ, Dixon RA. It’s never too late to engage in lifestyle activities: significant concurrent but not change relationships between lifestyle activities and cognitive speed. Journals of Gerontology. 2007;62(6):P331–P339.
    1. Small BJ, Dixon RA, McArdle JJ, Grimm KJ. Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:144–155.
    1. Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(5):634–642.
    1. Ghisletta P, Bickel JF, Lövdén M. Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. Journals of Gerontology. 2006;61(5):P253–P261.
    1. Wang JYJ, Zhou DHD, Li J, et al. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing aging study. Neurology. 2006;66(6):911–913.
    1. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Gilley DW, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Evans DA. Premorbid reading activity and patterns of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology. 2000;57(12):1718–1723.
    1. Fritsch T, McClendon MJ, Smyth KA, Lerner AJ, Friedland RP, Larsen JD. Cognitive functioning in healthy aging: the role of reserve and lifestyle factors early in life. Gerontologist. 2007;47(3):307–322.
    1. Kliegel M, Zimprich D, Rott C. Life-long intellectual activities mediate the predictive effect of early education on cognitive impairment in centenarians: a retrospective study. Aging and Mental Health. 2004;8(5):430–437.
    1. Fried LP, Carlson MC, Freedman M, et al. A social model for health promotion for an aging population: initial evidence on the Experience Corps model. Journal of Urban Health. 2004;81(1):64–78.
    1. Rebok GW, Carlson MC, Glass TA, et al. Short-term impact of Experience Corps participation on children and schools: results from a pilot randomized trial. Journal of Urban Health. 2004;81(1):79–93.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–198.
    1. Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ. Wide Range Achievement Test-Fourth Edition. Lutz, Fla, USA: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2006.
    1. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist. 1986;5(1-2):165–173.
    1. Parisi JM, Rebok GW, Seeman TE, Tanner EK, Tan E, Fried LP. Lifestyle activities in sociodemographically at-risk urban, older adults prior to participation in the Baltimore Experience Corps trial. Activities, Adaptation, and Aging. In press.
    1. Jopp D, Hertzog C. Activities, self-referent memory beliefs, and cognitive performance: evidence for direct and mediated relations. Psychology and Aging. 2007;22(4):811–825.
    1. Parisi JM, Stine-Morrow EA, Noh SR, Morrow DG. Predispositional engagement, activity engagement, and cognition among older adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. 2009;16(4):485–504.
    1. Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27(5):763–776.
    1. Rey A. L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie tramatique. Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:286–340.
    1. Schmidt M. Rey Auditory and Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook. Los Angeles, Calif, USA: Western Psychological Services; 2004.
    1. MacKinnon DP. Contrasts in multiple mediator models. In: Rose JS, Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, editors. Multivariate Applications in Substance Use Research: New Methods for New Questions. Mahwah, NJ, USA: Erlbaum; 2000. pp. 141–160.
    1. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40(3):879–891.
    1. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(1):83–104.
    1. Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(4):422–445.
    1. Williams J, MacKinnon DP. Resampling and distribution of the product methods for testing indirect effects in complex models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2008;15(1):23–51.
    1. Arbuckle TY, Gold DP, Chaikelson JS, Lapidus S. Measurement of activity in the elderly: the activities checklist. Canadian Journal on Aging. 1994;13(4):550–565.
    1. Parisi JM. Engagement in adulthood: perceptions and participation in daily activities. Activities, Adaptation and Aging. 2010;34(1):1–16.
    1. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Science. 2003;14(2):125–130.
    1. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:173–196.
    1. Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve: implications for diagnosis and prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2004;4(5):374–380.
    1. Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes PB, Baltes MM, editors. Successful Aging: Perspectives from the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
    1. Lachman ME. Perceived control over aging-related declines: adaptive beliefs and behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15(6):282–286.
    1. Stine-Morrow EAL. The dumbledore hypothesis of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16(6):295–299.
    1. Soubelet A. Engaging in cultural activities compensates for educational differences in cognitive abilities. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2011;18:516–526.
    1. Gow AJ, Corley J, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Reverse causation in activity-cognitive ability associations: the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27:250–255.
    1. Schooler C, Mulatu MS, Oates G. The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging. 1999;14(3):483–506.
    1. Alzheimer’s Association. Alzheimer’s Disease Facts and Figures. Chicago, Ill, USA: Alzheimer’s Association; 2010.
    1. Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(3):341–348.
    1. Manly JJ, Touradji P, Tang MX, Stern Y. Literacy and memory decline among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(5):680–690.
    1. Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH, et al. Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;161(7):639–651.
    1. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;348(25):2508–2516.
    1. Wang HX, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;155(12):1081–1087.
    1. Andel R, Crowe M, Kareholt I, Wastesson J, Parker MG. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. Journals of Gerontology. 2011;66(3):287–291.
    1. Kramer AF, Bherer L, Colcombe SJ, Dong W, Greenough WT. Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. Journals of Gerontology. 2004;59(9):940–957.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren