Effects of Computerized Updating and Inhibition Training in Older Adults: The ACTOP Three-Arm Randomized Double-Blind Controlled Trial

Arnaud Boujut, Lynn Valeyry Verty, Samantha Maltezos, Maxime Lussier, Samira Mellah, Louis Bherer, Sylvie Belleville, Arnaud Boujut, Lynn Valeyry Verty, Samantha Maltezos, Maxime Lussier, Samira Mellah, Louis Bherer, Sylvie Belleville

Abstract

Background: Working memory (WM) capacity declines with advancing age, which impacts the ability to carry out complex cognitive activities in everyday life. Updating and inhibition processes have been identified as some of the most critical attentional control processes of WM and are linked to age-related WM decline. The general aim of the Attentional Control Training in Older People (ACTOP) study was to perform a side-by-side comparison of updating and inhibition training to examine their respective efficacy and transfer in cognitively healthy older adults. Method: The study was a three-arm, double-blind, randomized controlled trial registered with the US National Institutes of Health clinical trials registry. Ninety older adults were randomly assigned to 12 half-hour sessions of updating (N-back type exercises), inhibition (Stroop-like exercises) computerized training or active control (general knowledge quiz game). A group of thirty younger adults completed all proximal and WM transfer tasks without training to assess age-related deficits prior to training and whether training reduces these deficits. Results: Piecewise mixed models show quick improvement of performance during training for both updating and inhibition training. During updating training, the progression was more pronounced for the most difficult (3-back) than for the least (1-back) difficult level until the ninth session. Updating and inhibition training groups improved performance on all proximal and WM transfer measures but these improvements did not differ from the active control group. Younger adults outperformed older ones on all transfer tasks prior to training. However, this was no longer the case following training for two transfer tasks regardless of the training group. Conclusion: The overall results from this study suggest that attentional control training is effective in improving updating and inhibition performance on training tasks. The optimal dose to achieve efficacy is ~9 half-hour sessions and the dose effect was related to difficulty level for updating training. Despite an overall improvement of older adults on all transfer tasks, neither updating nor inhibition training provided additional improvements in comparison with the active control condition. This suggests that the efficacy of process-based training does not directly affect transfer tasks. Clinical Trial Registration: www.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier: NCT03532113.

Keywords: aging; attentional control; cognitive training; transfer; working memory.

Conflict of interest statement

SB has been a consultant for research development on the prevention of Alzheimer disease for the Fondation IUGM (2016) and for Sojecci (2017 to current), and for the development of a cognitive stimulation program for the Centre de promotion de la Santé AvantÂge (2015). She has intellectual property rights on the “Programme de Stimulation pour une santé cognitive, Memoria, Batterie d'évaluation de la mémoire Côte-des-Neiges” and “MEMO, Méthode d'Entrainement pour une Mémoire Optimale.” She collaborates and receives funding from Mind Maze and Beam Me Up. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Copyright © 2020 Boujut, Verty, Maltezos, Lussier, Mellah, Bherer and Belleville.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Flow diagram indicating participant progress throughout the trial.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Illustration of the attentional control training. (A) Example of an N-back exercise used for updating training. In this 1-back trial, the planet symbol does not match the previously displayed moon symbol. (B) Two examples of a Stroop-like exercise used for inhibition training. In these incongruent trials, the correct response is “three” for the example shown above and “five” for the example below.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Comparison of performances (IES) by difficulty level throughout updating (A) and inhibition (B) training programs segmented by groups of three sessions. The Inverse Efficiency Score (IES) corresponds to the mean reaction time from each training session divided by the proportion of accuracy. Solid lines correspond to the average performance and colored dashed lines correspond to the regression slopes segmented into four segments of three training sessions representing the four time segments of training. The dashed black vertical line depicts the end of each training segment, where the slopes are differentiated. The error bars depict the SEM.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Performance growth on proximal transfer outcomes as a function of time and training group. Composite scores correspond to the averaged z scores of the antisaccade and Victoria Stroop tasks for the inhibition composite score (A), and the averaged z scores of the keep track and running span tasks for the updating composite score (B).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Performance growth on the complex WM outcomes as a function of time and intervention group. Correct responses in the reading span task (A) and accuracy rate in the alpha span task (B) are reported following a z score transformation. Composite scores in the dual virtual reality task (C) correspond to the mean between the z scores obtained on the verbal memory performance and the visual detection performance.

References

    1. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering M. Preventing Cognitive Decline and Dementia In: Leshner AI, Landis S, Stroud C, Downey A. editors. Preventing Cognitive Decline and Dementia: A Way Forward. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; (2017).
    1. Verhaeghen P. Working Memory and Cognitive Aging. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology; Available online at: (Retrieved November 20, 2020).
    1. Vaughan L, Giovanello K. Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychol Aging. (2010) 25:343–55. 10.1037/a0017729
    1. Engle RW. Working memory and executive attention: a revisit. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2018) 13:190–3. 10.1177/1745691617720478
    1. Friedman NP, Miyake A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex. (2017) 86:186–204. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
    1. Sylvain-Roy S, Lungu O, Belleville S. Normal aging of the attentional control functions that underlie working memory. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2015) 70:698–708. 10.1093/geronb/gbt166
    1. Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science. (2008) 320:1510–2. 10.1126/science.1155466
    1. Kühn S, Lorenz RC, Weichenberger M, Becker M, Haesner M, O'Sullivan J, et al. . Taking control! Structural and behavioural plasticity in response to game-based inhibition training in older adults. NeuroImage. (2017) 156:199–206. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.026
    1. Nguyen L, Murphy K, Andrews G. Immediate and long-term efficacy of executive functions cognitive training in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2019) 145:698–733. 10.1037/bul0000196
    1. Du X, Ji Y, Chen T, Tang Y, Han B. Can working memory capacity be expanded by boosting working memory updating efficiency in older adults? Psychol Aging. (2018) 33:1134–51. 10.1037/pag0000311
    1. Heinzel S, Schulte S, Onken J, Duong Q-L, Riemer TG, Heinz A, et al. . Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. (2014) 21:146–73. 10.1080/13825585.2013.790338
    1. Li S-C, Schmiedek F, Huxhold O, Röcke C, Smith J, Lindenberger U. Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychol Aging. (2008) 23:731–42. 10.1037/a0014343
    1. Salminen T, Frensch P, Strobach T, Schubert T. Age-specific differences of dual n -back training. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. (2016) 23:18–39. 10.1080/13825585.2015.1031723
    1. Ji Y, Wang J, Chen T, Du X, Zhan Y. Plasticity of inhibitory processes and associated far-transfer effects in older adults. Psychol Aging. (2016) 31:415–29. 10.1037/pag0000102
    1. Tusch ES, Alperin BR, Ryan E, Holcomb PJ, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Changes in neural activity underlying working memory after computerized cognitive training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. (2016) 8:255. 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00255
    1. Xin Z, Lai Z-R, Li F, Maes JHR. Near- and far-transfer effects of working memory updating training in elderly adults. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2014) 28:403–8. 10.1002/acp.3011
    1. Lawlor-Savage L, Goghari VM. Dual N-back working memory training in healthy adults: a randomized comparison to processing speed training. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0151817. 10.1371/journal.pone.0151817
    1. Bier B, De Boysson C, Belleille S. Identifying training modalities to improve multitasking in older adults. Age. (2014) 36:9688. 10.1007/s11357-014-9688-2
    1. Bier B, Ouellet É, Belleville S. Computerized attentional training and transfer with virtual reality: Effect of age and training type. Neuropsychology. (2018) 32:597–614. 10.1037/neu0000417
    1. Belleville S, Gilbert B, Fontaine F, Gagnon L, Ménard É, Gauthier S. Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dement Geriat Cogn Disord. (2006) 22:486–99. 10.1159/000096316
    1. Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M. Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. (2014) 11:e1001756. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756
    1. Boujut A, Mellah S, Lussier M, Maltezos S, Verty LV, Bherer L, et al. . Assessing the effect of training on the cognition and brain of older adults: protocol for a three-arm randomized double-blind controlled trial (ACTOP). JMIR Res Protoc. (2020) 9. 10.2196/20430
    1. Lussier M, Brouillard P, Bherer L. Limited benefits of heterogeneous dual-task training on transfer effects in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2017) 72:801–12. 10.1093/geronb/gbv105
    1. Lussier M, Bugaiska A, Bherer L. Specific transfer effects following variable priority dual-task training in older adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci. (2017) 35:237–50. 10.3233/RNN-150581
    1. Lussier M, Gagnon C, Bherer L. An investigation of response and stimulus modality transfer effects after dual-task training in younger and older. Front Hum Neurosci. (2012) 6:129. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00129
    1. Yntema DB. Keeping Track of Several Things at Once. Human Factors. (1963) 5:7–17. 10.1177/001872086300500102
    1. Tremblay MP, Potvin O, Belleville S, Bier N, Gagnon L, Blanchet S, et al. . The victoria stroop test: normative data in quebec-french adults and elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2016) 31:926–33. 10.1093/arclin/acw029
    1. Roberts RJ, Hager LD, Heron C. Prefrontal cognitive processes: working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. J Exp Psychol Gen. (1994) 123:374–93. 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.374
    1. Belleville S, Rouleau N, Caza N. Effect of normal aging on the manipulation of information in working memory. Mem Cognit. (1998) 26:572–83. 10.3758/BF03201163
    1. de Ribaupierre A, Ludwig C. Age differences and divided attention: is there a general deficit? Exp Aging Res. (2003) 29:79–105. 10.1080/03610730303705
    1. Rami L, Valls-Pedret C, Bartrés-Faz D, Caprile C, Solé-Padullés C, Castellvi M, et al. . [Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer's disease]. Rev Neurol. (2011) 52:195–201. 10.33588/rn.5204.2010478
    1. Hachinski VC, Iliff LD, Zilhka E, Du Boulay GH, McAllister VL, Marshall J, et al. . Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Arch Neurol. (1975) 32:632–7. 10.1001/archneur.1975.00490510088009
    1. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. 9/geriatric depression scale (Gds) recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol. (1986) 5:165–73. 10.1300/J018v05n01_09
    1. Larouche E, Tremblay M-P, Potvin O, Laforest S, Bergeron D, Laforce R, et al. . Normative data for the montreal cognitive assessment in middle-aged and elderly quebec-french people. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2016) 31:819–26. 10.1093/arclin/acw076
    1. Belleville S, LeBlanc AC, Kergoat M, Calon F, Gaudreau P, Hébert SS, et al. The consortium for the early identification of alzheimer's disease–quebec (CIMA-Q). Alzheimer Dement. (2019) 11:787–96. 10.1016/j.dadm.2019.07.003
    1. Elwood RW. The wechsler memory scale-revised: psychometric characteristics and clinical application. Neuropsychol Rev. (1991) 2:179–201. 10.1007/BF01109053
    1. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Trials. (2010) 11:1–8. 10.1186/1745-6215-11-32
    1. Gupta S. Intention-to-treat concept: a review. Perspect Clin Res. (2011) 2:109. 10.4103/2229-3485.83221
    1. Meteyard L, Davies RAI. Best practice guidance for linear mixed-effects models in psychological science. J Mem Lang. (2020) 112:104092 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104092
    1. Webb SL, Loh V, Lampit A, Bateman JE, Birney DP. Meta-analysis of the effects of computerized cognitive training on executive functions: a cross-disciplinary taxonomy for classifying outcome cognitive factors. Neuropsychol Rev. (2018) 28:232–50. 10.1007/s11065-018-9374-8
    1. Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence: maintenance and disengagement. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2016) 11:771–99. 10.1177/1745691616650647
    1. Boujut A, Belleville S. Où en est-on avec les programmes d'interventions cognitives pour les personnes âgées? Rev Neuropsychol. (2019) 11:60 10.3917/rne.111.0060

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren