Relationship between spectrotemporal modulation detection and music perception in normal-hearing, hearing-impaired, and cochlear implant listeners

Ji Eun Choi, Jong Ho Won, Cheol Hee Kim, Yang-Sun Cho, Sung Hwa Hong, Il Joon Moon, Ji Eun Choi, Jong Ho Won, Cheol Hee Kim, Yang-Sun Cho, Sung Hwa Hong, Il Joon Moon

Abstract

The objective of this study was to examine the relationship between spectrotemporal modulation (STM) sensitivity and the ability to perceive music. Ten normal-nearing (NH) listeners, ten hearing aid (HA) users with moderate hearing loss, and ten cochlear Implant (CI) users participated in this study. Three different types of psychoacoustic tests including spectral modulation detection (SMD), temporal modulation detection (TMD), and STM were administered. Performances on these psychoacoustic tests were compared to music perception abilities. In addition, psychoacoustic mechanisms involved in the improvement of music perception through HA were evaluated. Music perception abilities in unaided and aided conditions were measured for HA users. After that, HA benefit for music perception was correlated with aided psychoacoustic performance. STM detection study showed that a combination of spectral and temporal modulation cues were more strongly correlated with music perception abilities than spectral or temporal modulation cues measured separately. No correlation was found between music perception performance and SMD threshold or TMD threshold in each group. Also, HA benefits for melody and timbre identification were significantly correlated with a combination of spectral and temporal envelope cues though HA.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Psychoacoutic performances of each subject. Results of detection thresholds (dB) for each stimuli condition for NH listeners, HA users, and CI users are shown in circle (•), square (■), and triangle (▲), respectively. Spectral modulation detection (SMD) threshold (A) and temporal modulation detection (TMD) threshold (B) are shown in the upper row. Spectrotemoral modulation (STM) detection thresholds (C) are shown in the lower row. STM detection thresholds for spectral rates of 0.5, 1.0, and 2.0 c/o are shown in the left, middle, and right columns, respectively. Bars and error bars represent mean detection thresholds and standard deviation. Asterisk (*) indicates significant difference between two groups in post-hoc analysis (adjusted P-value was 0.05/3 based on Bonferroni correction).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Music perception abilities for each subject. Results of music perception abilities for NH listeners, HA users, and CI users are shown in circle (•), square (■), and triangle (▲), respectively. Bars and error bars represent mean abilities and standard deviation. Asterisk (*) indicates significant difference between two groups in post-hoc analysis (adjusted P-value was 0.05/3 based on Bonferroni correction).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Scatter plots of mean spectrotemporal modulation (STM) detection thresholds and music abilities. X-axis represents music perception abilities. Y-axis represents mean STM detection test. Mean STM detection thresholds defines averaged thresholds across six different stimuli conditions. Results of scatter plots for NH listeners, HA users, and CI users are shown in green circle (•), blue squared (■), and red triangle (▲), respectively. Panel A indicates pitch discrimination scores. Panel B indicates melody identification scores. Panel C indicates timbre identification sores.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Scatter plots of difference in music perception performances between aided and unaided conditions and aided psychoacoustic performances. X-axis represents psychoacoustic thresholds. Y-axis represents difference in music perception between unaided and aided conditions. If music perception was improved after wearing HA, values of Y-axis are positive. Values of X-axis going to the right side means better psychoacoustic performance. Red circle indicates subjects with better unaided scores than limit of 95% confidence interval (1.35 semitones for pitch-direction discrimination, 78% for melody identification scores, and 55% for timbre identification scores).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Audiograms for normal hearing (NH) listeners, hearing aid (HA) users, and cochlear implant (CI) users. Panel A shows audiograms for NH listeners. Pure tone thresholds are shown in circle (⚪) for tested ear and in square (□) for non-tested ear. NH listeners had pure tone thresholds better than or equal to 25 dB HL at all frequencies in both ears. Panel B shows audiograms for HA users. Aided pure tone thresholds are shown in black circle (•) for both ears. Unaided pure tone thresholds are shown in circle (⚪) for the tested ear and in square (□) for the non-tested ear. HA users had pure tone average worse than or equal to 40 dB HL for 500 Hz, 1,000 Hz, 2,000 Hz, and 3,000 Hz in both ears. Panel C shows audiograms for CI users. Aided pure tone thresholds are shown in black circle (•) for the tested ear.

References

    1. Wilson BS, Dorman MF. Cochlear implants: a remarkable past and a brilliant future. Hear Res. 2008;242:3–21. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2008.06.005.
    1. Hallgren M, Larsby B, Lyxell B, Arlinger S. Speech understanding in quiet and noise, with and without hearing aids. Int J Audiol. 2005;44:574–583. doi: 10.1080/14992020500190011.
    1. Madsen, S. M. & Moore, B. C. Music and hearing aids. Trends Hear18 (2014).
    1. Looi V, She J. Music perception of cochlear implant users: a questionnaire, and its implications for a music training program. Int J Audiol. 2010;49:116–128. doi: 10.3109/14992020903405987.
    1. Uys M, van Dijk C. Development of a music perception test for adult hearing-aid users. S Afr J Commun Disord. 2011;58:19–47.
    1. Marshall, C. Hear the music… or not? Hearing Journal57 (2004).
    1. Looi V, McDermott H, McKay C, Hickson L. Music perception of cochlear implant users compared with that of hearing aid users. Ear Hear. 2008;29:421–434. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31816a0d0b.
    1. Kirchberger, M. & Russo, F. A. Dynamic Range Across Music Genres and the Perception of Dynamic Compression in Hearing-Impaired Listeners. Trends Hear20 (2016).
    1. Kiefer J, Hohl S, Sturzebecher E, Pfennigdorff T, Gstoettner W. Comparison of speech recognition with different speech coding strategies (SPEAK, CIS, and ACE) and their relationship to telemetric measures of compound action potentials in the nucleus CI 24M cochlear implant system. Audiology. 2001;40:32–42. doi: 10.3109/00206090109073098.
    1. Drennan WR, Rubinstein JT. Music perception in cochlear implant users and its relationship with psychophysical capabilities. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2008;45:779–789. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2007.08.0118.
    1. Won JH, Drennan WR, Kang RS, Rubinstein JT. Psychoacoustic abilities associated with music perception in cochlear implant users. Ear Hear. 2010;31:796–805. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e8b7bd.
    1. Jung KH, et al. Psychoacoustic performance and music and speech perception in prelingually deafened children with cochlear implants. Audiol Neurootol. 2012;17:189–197. doi: 10.1159/000336407.
    1. Kong YY, Cruz R, Jones JA, Zeng FG. Music perception with temporal cues in acoustic and electric hearing. Ear Hear. 2004;25:173–185. doi: 10.1097/01.AUD.0000120365.97792.2F.
    1. Won JH, et al. Spectrotemporal Modulation Detection and Speech Perception by Cochlear Implant Users. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0140920. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140920.
    1. Chi T, Gao Y, Guyton MC, Ru P, Shamma S. Spectro-temporal modulation transfer functions and speech intelligibility. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1999;106:2719–2732. doi: 10.1121/1.428100.
    1. Supin A, Popov VV, Milekhina ON, Tarakanov MB. Frequency-temporal resolution of hearing measured by rippled noise. Hear Res. 1997;108:17–27. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(97)00035-X.
    1. Jung KH, et al. Clinical assessment of music perception in Korean cochlear implant listeners. Acta Otolaryngol. 2010;130:716–723. doi: 10.3109/00016480903380521.
    1. Drennan WR, Longnion JK, Ruffin C, Rubinstein JT. Discrimination of Schroeder-phase harmonic complexes by normal-hearing and cochlear-implant listeners. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2008;9:138–149. doi: 10.1007/s10162-007-0107-6.
    1. Plack, C. J. In The Sense of Hearing: Second Edition 213–233 (Routledge, 2016).
    1. Smith ZM, Delgutte B, Oxenham AJ. Chimaeric sounds reveal dichotomies in auditory perception. Nature. 2002;416:87–90. doi: 10.1038/416087a.
    1. Patil K, Pressnitzer D, Shamma S, Elhilali M. Music in our ears: the biological bases of musical timbre perception. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8:e1002759. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002759.
    1. Choi JE, et al. Evaluation of Cochlear Implant Candidates using a Non-linguistic Spectrotemporal Modulation Detection Test. Sci Rep. 2016;6:35235. doi: 10.1038/srep35235.
    1. George EL, Goverts ST, Festen JM, Houtgast T. Measuring the effects of reverberation and noise on sentence intelligibility for hearing-impaired listeners. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53:1429–1439. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0197).
    1. Zahorik P, et al. Amplitude modulation detection by human listeners in sound fields. Proc Meet Acoust. 2011;12:50005–50010. doi: 10.1121/1.3656342.
    1. Kang R, et al. Development and validation of the University of Washington Clinical Assessment of Music Perception test. Ear Hear. 2009;30:411–418. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181a61bc0.
    1. Anderson ES, Oxenham AJ, Nelson PB, Nelson DA. Assessing the role of spectral and intensity cues in spectral ripple detection and discrimination in cochlear-implant users. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2012;132:3925–3934. doi: 10.1121/1.4763999.
    1. Zhang T, Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Saoji A. Relationship between auditory function of nonimplanted ears and bimodal benefit. Ear Hear. 2013;34:133–141. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31826709af.
    1. Saoji AA, Litvak L, Spahr AJ, Eddins DA. Spectral modulation detection and vowel and consonant identifications in cochlear implant listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2009;126:955–958. doi: 10.1121/1.3179670.
    1. Eddins DA, Bero EM. Spectral modulation detection as a function of modulation frequency, carrier bandwidth, and carrier frequency region. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2007;121:363–372. doi: 10.1121/1.2382347.
    1. Won JH, Drennan WR, Nie K, Jameyson EM, Rubinstein JT. Acoustic temporal modulation detection and speech perception in cochlear implant listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2011;130:376–388. doi: 10.1121/1.3592521.
    1. Zheng Y, Escabi M, Litovsky RY. Spectro-temporal cues enhance modulation sensitivity in cochlear implant users. Hear Res. 2017;351:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.05.009.
    1. Landsberger, D. M., Padilla, M., Martinez, A. S. & Eisenberg, L. S. Spectral-Temporal Modulated Ripple Discrimination by Children With Cochlear Implants. Ear Hear (2017).
    1. Bernstein JG, et al. Spectrotemporal modulation sensitivity as a predictor of speech intelligibility for hearing-impaired listeners. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 2013;24:293–306. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.24.4.5.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren