Music playschool enhances children's linguistic skills

Tanja Linnavalli, Vesa Putkinen, Jari Lipsanen, Minna Huotilainen, Mari Tervaniemi, Tanja Linnavalli, Vesa Putkinen, Jari Lipsanen, Minna Huotilainen, Mari Tervaniemi

Abstract

Several studies have suggested that intensive musical training enhances children's linguistic skills. Such training, however, is not available to all children. We studied in a community setting whether a low-cost, weekly music playschool provided to 5-6-year-old children in kindergartens could already affect their linguistic abilities. Children (N = 66) were tested four times over two school-years with Phoneme processing and Vocabulary subtests, along with tests for Perceptual reasoning skills and Inhibitory control. We compared the development of music playschool children to their peers either attending to similarly organized dance lessons or not attending to either activity. Music playschool significantly improved the development of children's phoneme processing and vocabulary skills. No such improvements on children's scores for non-verbal reasoning and inhibition were obtained. Our data suggest that even playful group music activities - if attended to for several years - have a positive effect on pre-schoolers' linguistic skills. Therefore we promote the concept of implementing regular music playschool lessons given by professional teachers in early childhood education.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Individual test scores for Phoneme processing and Vocabulary for all four test times. The lines in (a and b) represent for illustration purposes the development of scores over time for children participating in music playschool either for 18 or 0 months and in (d) for children participating for 18 months either in music playschool or dance lessons, children participating in both activities for 18 months and children not participating in either activity. (c) The bars represent for illustration purposes the mean Vocabulary scores for children either not participating in dance lessons and having mother with low education (Low: scores 14.639, SEM = ±1.474) or participating in dance lessons for 27 months and having mother with high education (High: scores 17.534, SEM = ±1.138). Low mother’s education stands for 2 and high for 6 on the scale of 1 to 7.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean and individual scores for Perceptual reasoning index. (a) The bars represent for illustration purposes the mean Perceptual reasoning index scores for children either not participating in dance lessons and having mother with low education (Low: scores 28.626, SEM = ±2.823) or participating in dance lessons for 27 months and having mother with high education (High: scores 35.808, SEM = ±4.024). (b) Individual test scores for all four test times. The lines represent for illustration purposes the development of the scores for children participating in dance lessons for 27 or 0 months and having mother’s with high or low education. Low mother’s education stands for 2 and high for 6 on the scale of 1 to 7.

References

    1. Zuk J, et al. Enhanced syllable discrimination thresholds in musicians. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(12):e80546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080546.
    1. Du Y, Zatorre RJ. Musical training sharpens and bonds ears and tongue to hear speech better. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114(51):13579–13584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712223114.
    1. Parbery-Clark A, Skoe E, Lam C, Kraus N. Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear Hear. 2009;30(6):653–661. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9.
    1. Chan A, Ho Y, Cheung M. Music training improves verbal memory. Nature. 1998;396:128. doi: 10.1038/24075.
    1. Ho Y, Cheung M, Chan A. Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(3):439–450. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.439.
    1. Magne C, Schön D, Besson M. Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(2):199–211. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.2.199.
    1. Corrigall KA, Trainor LJ. Associations between lenght of music training and reading skills in children. Music Percept. 2011;29:147–55. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.147.
    1. Anvari S, Trainor L, Woodside J, Levy BA. Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. JExp Child Psychology. 2002;83(2):111–130. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00124-8.
    1. Forgeard M, Winner E, Norton A, Schlaug G. Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(10):e3566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003566.
    1. Slevc L, Miyake A. Individual differences in second-language proficiency: Does musical ability matter? Psychol Sci. 2006;17:674–681. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01765.x.
    1. Milovanov R, Pietilä P, Tervaniemi M, Esquef PA. Foreign language pronunciation skills and musical aptitude: A study of Finnish adults with higher education. Learn Individ Differ. 2010;20(1):56–60. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.11.003.
    1. Milovanov R, Huotilainen M, Välimäki V, Esquef PA, Tervaniemi M. Musical aptitude and second language pronunciation skills in school-aged children: neural and behavioral evidence. Brain Res. 2008;1194:81–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.042.
    1. Swaminathan S, Schellenberg EG. Musical competence and phoneme perception in a foreign language. Psychon Bull Rev. 2017;24(6):1929–1934. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1244-5.
    1. Bhatara A, Yeung HH, Nazzi T. Foreign language learning in French speakers is associated with rhythm perception, but not with melody perception. Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2015;41(2):277–282. doi: 10.1037/a0038736.
    1. Marques C, Moreno S, Castro SI, Besson M. Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than nonmusicians: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(9):1453–1463. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1453.
    1. Yang H, Ma W, Gong D, Hu J, Yao D. A longitudinal study on children’s music training experience and academic development. Sci Rep. 2014;4:5854. doi: 10.1038/srep05854.
    1. François C, Chobert J, Besson M, Schön D. Music training for the development of speech segmentation. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(9):2038–2043. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs180.
    1. Roden I, Kreutz G, Bongard S. Effects of a school-based instrumental music program on verbal and visual memory in primary school children: a longitudinal study. Front Neurosci. 2012;3:572.
    1. Slater J, et al. Longitudinal effects of group music instruction on literacy skills in low-income children. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(11):e113383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113383.
    1. Moritz C, Yampolsky S, Papadelis K, Thomson J, Wolf M. Links between early rhythm skills, musical training, and phonological awareness. Read Writ. 2013;26(5):739–769. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9389-0.
    1. Rautenberg I. The effects of musical training on the decoding skills of German-speaking primary school children. J Res Read. 2015;38(1):1–17. doi: 10.1111/jrir.12010.
    1. Flaugnacco E, et al. Music training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental dyslexia: a randomized control trial. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(9):e0138715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138715.
    1. Moreno S, et al. M. Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(3):712–723. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn120.
    1. Degé F, Schwarzer G. The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Front Psychol. 2011;2:124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124.
    1. Cogo-Moreira H, de Ávila CRB, Ploubidis GB, Mari JD. Effectiveness of music education for the improvement of reading skills and academic achievement in young poor readers: a pragmatic cluster-randomized, controlled clinical trial. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(3):e59984. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059984.
    1. Overy K. Dyslexia and music: from timing deficits to musical intervention. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2003;999:497–505. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.060.
    1. Bhide A, Power A, Goswami U. A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: a comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention. Mind Brain Educ. 2013;7:113–123. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12016.
    1. Fonseca-Mora M, Jara-Jiménez P, Gómez-Domínguez M. Musical plus phonological input for young foreign language readers. Front Psychol. 2015;6:286. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00286.
    1. Moreno S, et al. Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(11):1425–1433. doi: 10.1177/0956797611416999.
    1. dos Santos-Luiz C, Mónico LS, Almeida LS, Coimbra D. Exploring the long-term associations between adolescents’ music training and academic achievement. Music Sci. 2016;20(4):512–527. doi: 10.1177/1029864915623613.
    1. Schellenberg EG, Mankarious M. Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood. Emotion. 2012;12(5):887–891. doi: 10.1037/a0027971.
    1. Trimmer C, Cuddy L. Emotional intelligence, not music training, predicts recognition of emotional speech prosody. Emotion. 2008;8(6):838–849. doi: 10.1037/a0014080.
    1. Schellenberg EG. Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. Brit J Psychol. 2011;102:283–302. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x.
    1. Degé F, Kubicek C, Schwarzer G. Music lessons and intelligence: a relation mediated by executive functions. Music Percept. 2011;29(2):195–201. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.195.
    1. Silvia P, Thomas K, Nussbaum E, Beaty R, Hodges D. How does music training predict cognitive abilities? A bifactor approach to musical expertise and intelligence. Psychol Aesthet Crea. 2016;10(2):184–190. doi: 10.1037/aca0000058.
    1. Bergman-Nutley S, Darki F, Klingberg T. Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:926.
    1. Swaminathan S, Schellenberg EG, Khalil S. Revisiting the association between music lessons and intelligence: Training effects or music aptitude? Intelligence. 2017;62:119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.03.005.
    1. Corrigall KA, Schellenberg EG, Misura NM. Music training, cognition, and personality. Front Psychol. 2013;4:222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222.
    1. Schellenberg EG. Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychol Sci. 2004;15(8):511–514. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x.
    1. Kaviani H, Mirbaha H, Pournaseh M, Sagan O. Can music lessons increase the performance of preschool children in IQ tests. Cogn Process. 2014;15(1):77–84. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0574-0.
    1. Bugos, J. & Jacobs, E. Composition instruction and cognitive performance: results of a pilot study. Res Issues Music Educ. 10(1), Art2 (2012).
    1. Schellenberg EG, Hallam S. Music Listening and Cognitive Abilities in 10- and 11-Year-Olds: The Blur Effect. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2005;1060:202–209. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.013.
    1. Mehr SA, Schachner A, Katz RC, Spelke ES. Two randomized trials provide no consistent evidence for nonmusical cognitive benefits of brief preschool music enrichment. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(12):e82007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082007.
    1. Miyake A, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychol. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.
    1. Zuk J, Benjamin C, Kenyon A, Gaab N. Behavioral and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(6):e99868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099868.
    1. Dumont E, Syurina E, Feron F, van Hooren S. Music interventions and child development: a critical review and further directions. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1694. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01694.
    1. Bugos J, DeMarie D. The effects of a short-term music program on preschool children’s executive functions. Psychol Music. 2017;45(6):855–867. doi: 10.1177/0305735617692666.
    1. Jaschke A, Eggermont L, Honing H, Scherder E. Music education and its effect on intellectual abilities in children: a systematic review. Rev Neurosci. 2013;24(6):665–675. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2013-0023.
    1. Jaschke, A. C., Honing, H. & Scherder, E. J. A. Longitudinal Analysis of Music Education on Executive Functions in Primary School Children. Front Neurosci12(103), 10.3389/fnins.2018.00103 (2018).
    1. Bradley R, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233.
    1. Kirschner S, Tomasello M. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol Hum Behav. 2010;31(5):354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004.
    1. Schellenberg EG, Corrigall KA, Dys SP, Malti T. Group music training and children’s prosocial skills. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0141449. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141449.
    1. Ritblatt S, Longstreth S, Hokoda A, Cannon B-N, Weston J. Can music enhance school-readiness socioemotional skills? J Res Child Educ. 2013;27(3):257–266. doi: 10.1080/02568543.2013.796333.
    1. Habibi, A. et al. Childhood music training induces change in micro and macroscopic brain structure: Results from a longitudinal study. Cereb Cortex. 1–12 (2017).
    1. Kraus N, et al. Music enrichment programs improve the neural encoding of speech in at-risk children. J Neurosci. 2014;34(36):11913–11918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1881-14.2014.
    1. Huotilainen, M. & Tervaniemi, M. Planning music-based amelioration and training in infancy and childhood based on neural evidence. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1–9 (2018).
    1. Ullén F, Hambrick DZ, Mosing MA. Rethinking expertise: A multifactorial gene-environment interaction model of expert performance. Psychol Bull. 2015;142(4):427–446. doi: 10.1037/bul0000033.
    1. Mets DG, Brainard MS. Genetic variation interacts with experience to determine interindividual differences in learned song. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115(2):421–426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713031115.
    1. Korkman, M., Kirk, U. & Kemp, S. L. Nepsy II − lasten neuropsykologinen tutkimus. 1st edition. Psykologien Kustannus Oy (2008).
    1. Wechsler, D. WISC-IV. Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children - IV. NCS Pearson, Ltd. Psykologien Kustannus Oy (2010).
    1. Brooks BL, E. M S, Strauss E. NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, Second Edition. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;16(1):80–101. doi: 10.1080/09297040903146966.
    1. Korkman, M., Kirk, U. & Kemp, S. L. Nepsy II: Clinical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2007).
    1. Kaufman AS, Flanagan DP, Alfonso VC, Mascolo JT. Test review: Wechsler intelligence scale for children, fourth edition (WISC-IV) J Psychoeduc Assess. 2006;24(3):278–295. doi: 10.1177/0734282906288389.
    1. West B. Analyzing longitudinal data with the linear mixed models procedure in SPSS. Eval Health Prof. 2009;32(3):207–228. doi: 10.1177/0163278709338554.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren