An investigation of reasoning by analogy in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder

Daniel C Krawczyk, Michelle R Kandalaft, Nyaz Didehbani, Tandra T Allen, M Michelle McClelland, Carol A Tamminga, Sandra B Chapman, Daniel C Krawczyk, Michelle R Kandalaft, Nyaz Didehbani, Tandra T Allen, M Michelle McClelland, Carol A Tamminga, Sandra B Chapman

Abstract

Relational reasoning ability relies upon by both cognitive and social factors. We compared analogical reasoning performance in healthy controls (HC) to performance in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), and individuals with schizophrenia (SZ). The experimental task required participants to find correspondences between drawings of scenes. Participants were asked to infer which item within one scene best matched a relational item within the second scene. We varied relational complexity, presence of distraction, and type of objects in the analogies (living or non-living items). We hypothesized that the cognitive differences present in SZ would reduce relational inferences relative to ASD and HC. We also hypothesized that both SZ and ASD would show lower performance on living item problems relative to HC due to lower social function scores. Overall accuracy was higher for HC relative to SZ, consistent with prior research. Across groups, higher relational complexity reduced analogical responding, as did the presence of non-living items. Separate group analyses revealed that the ASD group was less accurate at making relational inferences in problems that involved mainly non-living items and when distractors were present. The SZ group showed differences in problem type similar to the ASD group. Additionally, we found significant correlations between social cognitive ability and analogical reasoning, particularly for the SZ group. These results indicate that differences in cognitive and social abilities impact the ability to infer analogical correspondences along with numbers of relational elements and types of objects present in the problems.

Keywords: analogy; autism spectrum disorder; distraction; reasoning; schizophrenia.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
The examples of task problem types. The 24 problems included three factors that were varied factorially: number of relations (one-relation or two-relations), presence of distractor item (with or without distractor), and item type involved in the analogy (living or non-living items). Panel A shows an analogy with a single relation (reaching), involves living objects (people and a dog), and contains no distraction. Panel B consists of two relations (containing, and being contained), involves non-living objects (food, bowl, cabinet, groceries, bag, and car) with no distraction. Panel C is a one relation problem (containing) with non-living objects (water, pitcher, and bucket), with a distractor present (the pitcher in the lower picture). Panel D consists of two relations (receiving a message and relaying a message), living objects (people), and has a distractor (the woman in the lower picture).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Overall percent accuracy for each of the participant groups. HC participants showed greater overall performance than SZ participants. There was also a main effect of problem type and a main effect of relational complexity. (A) ASD individuals showed a reduction in performance when solving two-relation, non-living problems. (B) SZ showed reductions in performance for non-living problems and a marginal reduction in performance on two-relation problems. (C) HC participants showed high performance across all problems types.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th Edn. (text rev.). Washington, DC: Author
    1. Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., Plumb I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251 10.1111/1469-7610.00715
    1. Blackshaw A. J., Kinderman P., Hare D. J., Hatton C. (2001). Theory of mind, causal attribution and paranoia in Asperger syndrome. Autism 5, 147–163 10.1177/1362361301005002005
    1. Blair R. J. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to mortality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57, 1–29 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-p
    1. Bowman E. P. (1988). Asperger’s syndrome and autism: the case for a connection. British. Br. J. Psychiatry 152, 377–382 10.1192/bjp.152.3.377
    1. Cheng P. W., Holyoak K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cogn. Psychol. 17, 391–416 10.1016/0010-0285(85)90014-3
    1. Cho S., Holyoak K. J., Cannon T. D. (2007). Analogical reasoning in working memory: resources shared among relational integration, interference resolution and maintenance. Mem. Cognit. 35, 1445–1455 10.3758/bf03193614
    1. Chua H. F., Leu J., Nisbett R. E. (2005). Culture and diverging views of social events. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 925–934 10.1177/0146167204272166
    1. Cosmides L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31, 187–276 10.1016/0010-0277(89)90023-1
    1. Dickinson D., Iannone V. N., Wilk C. M., Gold J. M. (2004). General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 826–833 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)80916-7
    1. Doody G. A., Götz M., Johnstone E. C., Frith C. D., Owens D. G. (1998). Theory of mind and psychoses. Psychol. Med. 28, 397–405 10.1017/s003329179700648x
    1. Elliott R., McKenna P. J., Robbins T. W., Sahakian B. J. (1995). Neuropsychological evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 25, 619–630 10.1017/s0033291700033523
    1. Ermer E., Kiehl K. A. (2010). Psychopaths are impaired in social exchange and precautionary reasoning. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1399–1405 10.1177/0956797610384148
    1. Frith C. D., Corcoran R. (1996). Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 26, 521–530 10.1017/s0033291700035601
    1. Gentner D. (1983). Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cogn. Sci. Multidiscip. J. 7, 155–170 10.1207/s15516709cog0702_3
    1. Gentner D., Rattermann M. J. (1991). “Language and the career of similarity,” in Perspectives on Language and Thought: Interrelations in Development, eds Gelman S. A., Byrnes J. P. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; ), 225–277
    1. Gentner D., Toupin C. (1986). Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cogn. Sci. Multidiscip. J. 10, 277–300 10.1207/s15516709cog1003_2
    1. Gick M. L., Holyoak K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 1–38 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002-6
    1. Goel V., Stollstorff M., Nakic M., Knutson K., Grafman J. (2009). A role for right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in reasoning about indeterminate relations. Neuropsychologia 47, 2790–2797 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.002
    1. Goswami U. (2001). Cognitive development: no stages please—we’re British. Br. J. Psychol. 92, 257–277 10.1348/000712601162068
    1. Goswami U., Brown A. L. (1989). Melting chocolate and melting snowman. Analogical reasoning and causal relations. Cognition 35, 69–95 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90037-k
    1. Green M. F., Kern R. S., Braff D. L., Mintz J. (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr. Bull. 26, 119–136 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430
    1. Halford G. S., Maybery M. T., O’Hare A. W., Grant P. (1994). The development of memory and processing capacity. Child Dev. 65, 1338–1356 10.2307/1131502
    1. Hayashi M., Kato M., Igarashi K., Kashima H. (2008). Superior fluid intelligence in children with Asperger’s disorder. Brain Cogn. 66, 306–310 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.09.008
    1. Heinrichs R. W., Zakzanis K. K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426–445 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.426
    1. Holyoak K. J., Kroger J. K. (1995). “Forms of reasoning: insight into prefrontal functions?” in Structure and Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex, eds Grafman J., Holyoak K. J., Boller F. (New York: New York Academy of Sciences; ), 253–263
    1. Hummel J. E., Holyoak K. J. (1997). Distributed representations of structure: a theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychol. Rev. 104, 427–466 10.1037//0033-295x.104.3.427
    1. Hummel J. E., Holyoak K. J. (2003). A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychol. Rev. 110, 220–264 10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.220
    1. Kotovsky L., Gentner D. (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Dev. 67, 2797–2822 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01889.x
    1. Krawczyk D. C. (2012). The cognition and neuroscience of human reasoning. Brain Res. 1428, 13–23 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.080
    1. Krawczyk D. C., Hanten G., Wilde E. A., Li X., Schnelle K. P., Merkley T. L., et al. (2010). Deficits in analogical reasoning in adolescents with traumatic brain injury. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 62:62 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00062
    1. Krawczyk D. C., Holyoak K. J., Hummel J. E. (2005). The one-to-one constraint in analogical mapping and inference. Cogn. Sci. 29, 797–806 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_27
    1. Krawczyk D. C., Morrison R. G., Viskontas I., Holyoak K. J., Chow T. W., Mendez M. F., et al. (2008). Distraction during relational reasoning: the role of prefrontal cortex in interference control. Neuropsychologia 46, 2020–2032 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.001
    1. Lai M. C., Lombardo M. V., Baron-Cohen S. (2014). Autism. Lancet 6736, 61539–61541
    1. Langdon R., Ward P. B., Coltheart M. (2010). Reasoning anomalies associated with delusions in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 321–330 10.1093/schbul/sbn069
    1. Levin H. S., Hanten G. (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatr. Neurol. 33, 79–93 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.02.002
    1. Link N. F., Scherer S. E., Byrne P. N. (1977). Moral judgement and moral conduct in the psychopath. Can. Psychiatr. Assoc. J. 22, 341–346
    1. Markman A. B., Gentner D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 431–467 10.1006/cogp.1993.1011
    1. Matheson S., Langdon R. (2008). Schizotypal traits impact upon executive working memory and aspects of IQ. Psychiatry Res. 159, 207–214 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.006
    1. Morrison R. G., Krawczyk D. C., Holyoak K. J., Hummel J. E., Chow T. W., Miller B. L., et al. (2004). A neurocomputational model of analogical reasoning and its breakdown in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 260–271 10.1162/089892904322984553
    1. Morsanyi K., Holyoak K. J. (2010). Analogical reasoning ability in autistic and typically developing children. Dev. Sci. 13, 578–587 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00915.x
    1. Nuechterlein K. H., Barch D. M., Gold J. M., Goldberg T. E., Green M. F., Heaton R. K. (2004). Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 72, 29–39 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007
    1. Penn D. C., Holyoak K. J., Povinelli D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 109–130 10.1017/s0140525x08003543
    1. Pijnacker J., Geurts B., van Lambalgen M., Kan C. C., Buitelaar J. K., Hagoort P. (2009b). Defeasible reasoning in high-functioning adults with autism: evidence for impaired exception-handling. Neuropsychologia 47, 644–651 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.011
    1. Pijnacker J., Hagoort P., Buitelaar J., Teunisse J. P., Geurts B. (2009a). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 607–618 10.1007/s10803-008-0661-8
    1. Pinkham A. E., Hopfinger J. B., Pelphrey K. A., Piven J., Penn D. L. (2008). Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 99, 164–175 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.024
    1. Raine A., Yang Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 1, 203–213 10.1093/scan/nsl033
    1. Richland L. E., Morrison R. G., Holyoak K. J. (2006). Children’s development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 94, 249–273 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.02.002
    1. Sahyoun C. P., Soulières I., Belliveau J. W., Mottron L., Mody M. (2009). Cognitive differences in pictorial reasoning between high-functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1014–1023 10.1007/s10803-009-0712-9
    1. Sasson N. J., Pinkham A. E., Carpenter K. L. H., Belger A. (2011). The benefit of directly comparing autism and schizophrenia for revealing mechanisms of social cognitive impairment. J. Neurodev. Disord. 3, 87–100 10.1007/s11689-010-9068-x
    1. Shenhav A., Greene J. D. (2014). Integrative moral judgment: dissociating the roles of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 4741–4749 10.1523/jneurosci.3390-13.2014
    1. Simpson J., Done D. J. (2004). Analogical reasoning in schizophrenic delusions. Eur. Psychiatry 19, 344–348 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2004.05.009
    1. Snyder P. J., Jackson C. E., Piskulic D., Olver J., Norman T., Maruff P. (2008). Spatial working memory and problem solving in schizophrenia: the effect of symptom stabilization with atypical antipsychotic medication. Psychiatry Res. 160, 316–326 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.011
    1. Startup H., Freeman D., Garety P. A. (2008). Jumping to conclusions and persecutory delusions. Eur. Psychiatry 23, 457–459 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.04.005
    1. Tamminga C. A., Stan A. D., Wagner A. D. (2010). The hippocampal formation in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 1178–1193 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081187
    1. Tohill J. M., Holyoak K. J. (2000). The impact of anxiety on analogical reasoning. Think. Reason. 6, 27–40 10.1080/135467800393911
    1. Vendetti M., Knowlton B. J., Holyoak K. J. (2012). The impact of semantic distance and induced stress on analogical reasoning. A neurocomputational account. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 804–812 10.3758/s13415-012-0103-0
    1. Viskontas I. V., Morrison R. G., Holyoak K. J., Hummel J. E., Knowlton B. J. (2004). Relational integration, inhibition, and analogical reasoning in older adults. Psychol. Aging 19, 581–591 10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.581
    1. Waltz J. A., Knowlton B. J., Holyoak K. J., Boone K. B., Mishkin F. S., de Menezes Santos M., et al. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychol. Sci. 10, 119–125 10.1111/1467-9280.00118
    1. Waltz J. A., Lau A., Grewal S. K., Holyoak K. J. (2000). The role of working memory in analogical mapping. Mem. Cognit. 28, 1205–1212 10.3758/bf03211821
    1. Wharton C. M., Holyoak K. J., Downing P. E., Lange T. E., Wickens T. D., Melz E. R. (1994). Below the surface: analogical similarity and retrieval competition in reminding. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 64–101 10.1006/cogp.1994.1003
    1. Wharton C. M., Holyoak K. J., Lange T. E. (1996). Remote analogical reminding. Mem. Cognit. 24, 629–643 10.3758/bf03201088
    1. Ziv I., Leiser D., Levine J. (2011). Social cognition in schizophrenia: cognitive and affective factors. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 16, 71–91 10.1080/13546805.2010.492693

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren