Auditory Processing in Noise: A Preschool Biomarker for Literacy

Travis White-Schwoch, Kali Woodruff Carr, Elaine C Thompson, Samira Anderson, Trent Nicol, Ann R Bradlow, Steven G Zecker, Nina Kraus, Travis White-Schwoch, Kali Woodruff Carr, Elaine C Thompson, Samira Anderson, Trent Nicol, Ann R Bradlow, Steven G Zecker, Nina Kraus

Abstract

Learning to read is a fundamental developmental milestone, and achieving reading competency has lifelong consequences. Although literacy development proceeds smoothly for many children, a subset struggle with this learning process, creating a need to identify reliable biomarkers of a child's future literacy that could facilitate early diagnosis and access to crucial early interventions. Neural markers of reading skills have been identified in school-aged children and adults; many pertain to the precision of information processing in noise, but it is unknown whether these markers are present in pre-reading children. Here, in a series of experiments in 112 children (ages 3-14 y), we show brain-behavior relationships between the integrity of the neural coding of speech in noise and phonology. We harness these findings into a predictive model of preliteracy, revealing that a 30-min neurophysiological assessment predicts performance on multiple pre-reading tests and, one year later, predicts preschoolers' performance across multiple domains of emergent literacy. This same neural coding model predicts literacy and diagnosis of a learning disability in school-aged children. These findings offer new insight into the biological constraints on preliteracy during early childhood, suggesting that neural processing of consonants in noise is fundamental for language and reading development. Pragmatically, these findings open doors to early identification of children at risk for language learning problems; this early identification may in turn facilitate access to early interventions that could prevent a life spent struggling to read.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Overview of the auditory-neurophysiological biomarker…
Fig 1. Overview of the auditory-neurophysiological biomarker and three derived neural measures.
(A) Recording paradigm: [da] is presented repeatedly over a continuous background track of nonsense sentences spoken by multiple talkers. (B) A time-domain average waveform of the response. The response shows many of the physical characteristics of the eliciting stimulus. The gray box highlights the time region of the response that corresponds to the consonant transition (the region of interest). (C) The peaks of interest are identified here with arrows. (D) A frequency domain representation of the grand average response to the consonant transition. (E) To illustrate the trial-by-trial stability measure, two representative subjects are shown. One pair of sub-averages each is shown for a subject with high stability and one with poor stability (right).
Fig 2
Fig 2
(A) In Year 1 (Experiment 1) each child’s score on the phonological processing test is plotted against the model’s predicted scores (n = 37). The two are highly correlated (r = 0.826, p < .001; when a correction is applied for the unreliability of the psychoeducational test, r = 0.870, p < .001). (B) A histogram of the error of estimation (the difference between a preschooler’s actual and predicted scores). For a majority of children, the model predicts scores within 2 points on the test. Please refer to the S1 Data for data underlying this figure.
Fig 3. In preschoolers ( n =…
Fig 3. In preschoolers (n = 34), model predictions of phonological processing in Year 1 (based on auditory neurophysiology) predict rapid automatized naming time in Year 2, with higher predicted scores correlating with faster naming times for objects and colors (r = -.663, p < .001).
Please refer to the S1 Data for data underlying this figure.

References

    1. Centanni T, Booker A, Sloan A, Chen F, Maher B, Carraway R, et al. Knockdown of the dyslexia-associated gene Kiaa0319 impairs temporal responses to speech stimuli in rat primary auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24: 1753–1766. 10.1093/cercor/bht028
    1. Hornickel J, Kraus N. Unstable representation of sound: A biological marker of dyslexia. J Neurosci. 2013;33: 3500–3504. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4205-12.2013
    1. Banai K, Hornickel J, Skoe E, Nicol T, Zecker S, Kraus N. Reading and subcortical auditory function. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19: 2699–2707. 10.1093/cercor/bhp024
    1. Ahissar M, Protopapas A, Reid M, Merzenich MM. Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2000;97: 6832–6837.
    1. Tallal P. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain Lang. 1980;9: 182–198.
    1. Kraus N, McGee TJ, Carrell TD, Zecker SG, Nicol TG, Koch DB. Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science. 1996;273: 971–973.
    1. Kuhl PK. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5: 831–843.
    1. Goswami U. A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15: 3–10. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001
    1. Goswami U, Thomson J, Richardson U, Stainthorp R, Hughes D, Rosen S, et al. Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99: 10911–10916.
    1. Lehongre K, Ramus F, Villiermet N, Schwartz D, Giraud A-L. Altered low-gamma sampling in auditory cortex accounts for the three main facets of dyslexia. Neuron. 2011;72: 1080–1090. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.002
    1. Cunningham J, Nicol T, Zecker SG, Bradlow A, Kraus N. Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in children with learning problems: deficits and strategies for improvement. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112: 758–767.
    1. Sperling AJ, Lu Z-L, Manis FR, Seidenberg MS. Deficits in perceptual noise exclusion in developmental dyslexia. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8: 862–863.
    1. Ziegler JC, Pech-Georgel C, George F, Alario F-X, Lorenzi C. Deficits in speech perception predict language learning impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102: 14110–14115.
    1. Ziegler JC, Pech-Georgel C, George F, Lorenzi C. Speech-perception-in-noise deficits in dyslexia. Dev Sci. 2009;12: 732–745. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00817.x
    1. Anderson S, Skoe E, Chandrasekaran B, Kraus N. Neural timing is linked to speech perception in noise. J Neurosci. 2010;30: 4922–4926. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0107-10.2010
    1. Ahissar M, Lubin Y, Putter-Katz H, Banai K. Dyslexia and the failure to form a perceptual anchor. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9: 1558–1564.
    1. Nagarajan S, Mahncke H, Salz T, Tallal P, Roberts T, Merzenich MM. Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1999;96: 6483–6488.
    1. Rosen S. Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? J Phon. 2003;31: 509–527.
    1. Livingstone MS, Rosen GD, Drislane FW, Galaburda AM. Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1991;88: 7943–7947.
    1. Stein J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7: 12–36.
    1. Ahissar M. Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11: 458–465.
    1. Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52: 101–110.
    1. Castles A, Coltheart M. Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition. 2004;91: 77–111.
    1. Boets B. Dyslexia: reconciling controversies within an integrative developmental perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18: 501–503. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.003
    1. Pegado F, Comerlato E, Ventura F, Jobert A, Nakamura K, Buiatti M, et al. Timing the impact of literacy on visual processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2014;111: E5233–E5242. 10.1073/pnas.1417347111
    1. Kovelman I, Norton ES, Christodoulou JA, Gaab N, Lieberman DA, Triantafyllou C, et al. Brain basis of phonological awareness for spoken language in children and its disruption in dyslexia. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22: 754–764. 10.1093/cercor/bhr094
    1. Raschle NM, Stering PL, Meissner SN, Gaab N. Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cereb Cortex. 2013; 2489–2501.
    1. Clark KA, Helland T, Specht K, Narr KL, Manis FR, Toga AW, et al. Neuroanatomical precursors of dyslexia identified from pre-reading through to age 11. Brain. 2014;137: 3136–3141. 10.1093/brain/awu229
    1. Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA, Rose E, Lindamood P, Conway T, et al. Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. J Educ Psychol. 1999;91: 579.
    1. Foorman BR, Francis DJ, Fletcher JM, Schatschneider C, Mehta P. The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children. J Educ Psychol. 1998;90: 37.
    1. Bishop DV, Adams C. A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31: 1027–1050.
    1. Wright BA, Lombardino LJ, King WM, Puranik CS, Leonard CM, Merzenich MM. Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children. Nature. 1997;387: 176–178.
    1. Kraus N, Bradlow AR, Cheatham MA, Cunningham J, King CD, Koch DB, et al. Consequences of neural asynchrony: a case of auditory neuropathy. JARO-J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2000;1: 33–45.
    1. Landerl K, Ramus F, Moll K, Lyytinen H, Leppänen PH, Lohvansuu K, et al. Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54: 686–694. 10.1111/jcpp.12029
    1. McBride–Chang C, Kail RV. Cross–cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Dev. 2002;73: 1392–1407.
    1. Gathercole SE, Alloway TP, Willis C, Adams A-M. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol. 2006;93: 265–281.
    1. Papadimitriou AM, Vlachos FM. Which specific skills developing during preschool years predict the reading performance in the first and second grade of primary school? Early Child Dev Care. 2014; 1706–1722.
    1. Saygin ZM, Norton ES, Osher DE, Beach SD, Cyr AB, Ozernov-Palchik O, et al. Tracking the Roots of Reading Ability: White Matter Volume and Integrity Correlate with Phonological Awareness in Prereading and Early-Reading Kindergarten Children. J Neurosci. 2013;33: 13251–13258. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013
    1. Sprenger-Charolles L, Siegel LS, Béchennec D, Serniclaes W. Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. J Exp Child Psychol. 2003;84: 194–217.
    1. Boets B, de Beeck HPO, Vandermosten M, Scott SK, Gillebert CR, Mantini D, et al. Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science. 2013;342: 1251–1254. 10.1126/science.1244333
    1. Caspary DM, Ling L, Turner JG, Hughes LF. Inhibitory neurotransmission, plasticity and aging in the mammalian central auditory system. J Exp Biol. 2008;211: 1781–1791. 10.1242/jeb.013581
    1. Galaburda AM, Menard MT, Rosen GD. Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1994;91: 8010–8013.
    1. Platt M, Adler W, Mehlhorn A, Johnson G, Wright K, Choi R, et al. Embryonic disruption of the candidate dyslexia susceptibility gene homolog Kiaa0319-like results in neuronal migration disorders. Neuroscience. 2013;248: 585–593. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.06.056
    1. Finn ES, Shen X, Holahan JM, Scheinost D, Lacadie C, Papademetris X, et al. Disruption of functional networks in dyslexia: a whole-brain, data-driven analysis of connectivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2013; 397–404. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.031
    1. Hari R, Renvall H. Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5: 525–532.
    1. Olulade OA, Napoliello EM, Eden GF. Abnormal visual motion processing is not a cause of dyslexia. Neuron. 2013;79: 180–190. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.002
    1. Pugh KR, Frost SJ, Rothman DL, Hoeft F, Del Tufo SN, Mason GF, et al. Glutamate and Choline Levels Predict Individual Differences in Reading Ability in Emergent Readers. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 4082–4089. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3907-13.2014
    1. Molfese DL. Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain Lang. 2000;72: 238–245.
    1. Norton ES, Black JM, Stanley LM, Tanaka H, Gabrieli JD, Sawyer C, et al. Functional neuroanatomical evidence for the double-deficit hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2014;61: 235–246. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.015
    1. Benasich AA, Choudhury NA, Realpe-Bonilla T, Roesler CP. Plasticity in Developing Brain: Active Auditory Exposure Impacts Prelinguistic Acoustic Mapping. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 13349–13363. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0972-14.2014
    1. Orton LD, Poon PW, Rees A. Deactivation of the inferior colliculus by cooling demonstrates intercollicular modulation of neuronal activity. Front Neural Circuits. 2012;6.
    1. Skoe E, Kraus N. Auditory brain stem response to complex sounds: A tutorial. Ear Hear. 2010;31: 302–324. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181cdb272

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren