Child, Parent, and Play - An Insight into These Dimensions Among Children with and without Receptive Expressive Language Disorder Using Video-Based Analysis

Megha Mohan, Gagan Bajaj, Apramita Deshpande, Malavika Anakkathil Anil, Jayashree S Bhat, Megha Mohan, Gagan Bajaj, Apramita Deshpande, Malavika Anakkathil Anil, Jayashree S Bhat

Abstract

Purpose: Language development in children aged 3-6 years is shaped by their pre-linguistic abilities, communication patterns and play behaviors along with parental communicative roles. Little is known about how these aspects are distributed among children with receptive expressive language disorder (CWRELD) in comparison to typically developing children (CWTDL). The present research explores these differences between the two groups using a video-based analysis with a belief that an understanding of these aspects may facilitate age-appropriate speech and language acquisition in children with language delay.

Methods: A video-based analysis of parent-child interactions was carried out for 10 children each with receptive expressive language disorder and typical language development, respectively. The two groups were compared for the child's turn-taking skills, eye contact span, autonomous instances, communication patterns, play behaviors and parental communication roles.

Results: Children with receptive expressive language disorder exhibited significantly fewer proportion of turns and autonomous instances along with a greater proportion of eye contact to objects than the parent. Majority of the children with language delay were at the "Requester" or "Early communicators" stage and demonstrated either "Exploratory" or "Functional play" behaviors. Most of the typically developing children were at the "Partner stage" of communication and exhibited "Functional", "Constructive" or "Symbolic Play". Parents of children with language delay mostly exhibited "Helper" type communicative roles while parents from typically developing groups showed "Partner" type communicative profile.

Conclusion: A quantitative planning and monitoring of pre-linguistic skills, determination of communicative patterns and play behaviors is important for clinicians working with children having a language delay. Assessing and modifying parental communicative roles are also crucial. Understanding the distribution of these research variables among CWRELD in comparison to CWTDL may help clinicians in planning precise treatment goals, monitoring specific linguistic progress, ensuring better parental participation and delivering better outcomes during language therapy.

Keywords: communication stages; parental communication roles; parent–child interaction; play behaviors; pre-linguistic skills; receptive expressive language disorder.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work, financial or otherwise.

© 2021 Mohan et al.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Differences in pre-linguistic skills among CWRELD and CWTDL. The error bars represent standard deviation.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Distribution of parental roles across CWTDL and CWRELD groups.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Distribution of communication stages across CWTDL and CWRELD groups.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Distribution of play behaviors across CWRELD and CWTDL groups.

References

    1. Topping K, Dekhinet R, Zeedyk S. Parent-infant interaction and children’s language development. Educ Psychol. 2013;33(4):391–426. doi:10.1080/01443410.2012.744159
    1. Hadley PA, Holt JK. Individual differences in the onset of tense marking: a growth-curve analysis. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(5):984–1000. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/071)
    1. Patton OT, Kevin RA, Catherine SE. Home language and literacy environment: final results. - PsycNET. In: Dickinson D, editor. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Paul H Brookes Publishing; 2001:111–138.
    1. Page Melissa M, Wilhelm MS, Gamble WC, Card NA. A comparison of maternal sensitivity and verbal stimulation as unique predictors of infant social-emotional and cognitive development. Infant Behav Dev. 2010;33(1):101–110. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.12.001
    1. Watt N, Wetherby A, Shumway S. Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(6):1224–1237. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/088)
    1. Kelly-Vance L, Ryalls BO. Best practices in play assessment and intervention. Best Pract Sch Psychol V. 2004;549–560.
    1. Sameroff AJ, Fiese BH. Models of development and developmental risk. - PsycNET. In: Zeanah CHJ, editor. Handbook of Infant Mental Health. The Guilford Press; 2000:3–19.
    1. Van Kleeck A, Gillam RB, Hamilton L, McGrath C. The relationship between middle class parents’ book-sharing discussion and their preschoolers’ abstract language development. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40(6):1261–1271. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4006.1261
    1. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L. Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Dev. 2001;72(3):748–767. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00313
    1. Marshall J, Goldbart J, Phillips J. Parents’ and speech and language therapists’ explanatory models of language development, language delay and intervention. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42(5):533–555. doi:10.1080/13682820601053753
    1. Saracho ON. Family literacy: exploring family practices. Early Child Dev Care. 2002;172(2):113–122. doi:10.1080/03004430210886
    1. Silvén M, Niemi P, Voeten MJM. Do maternal interaction and early language predict phonological awareness in 3- to 4-year-olds? Cogn Dev. 2002;17(1):1133–1155. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00093-X
    1. Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27(2):155–168. doi:10.1097/00004703-200604000-00014
    1. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Assel MA, Vellet S. Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children’s development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Dev Psychol. 2001;37(3):387–403. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.387
    1. Sussman F. More Than Words. . The Hanen Centre; 2012:424.
    1. Garcia OF, Serra E, Zacares JJ, Calafat A, Garcia F. Alcohol use and abuse and motivations for drinking and non-drinking among Spanish adolescents: do we know enough when we know parenting style? Psychol Health. 2020;35(6):645–664. doi:10.1080/08870446.2019.1675660
    1. Sahithya BR, Manohari SM, Vijaya R. Parenting styles and its impact on children – a cross cultural review with a focus on India. Ment Health Relig Cult. 2019;22(4):357–383. doi:10.1080/13674676.2019.1594178
    1. Darling N, Steinberg L. Parenting styles context: an integrative model. Psychol Bull. 1993;113(3):487–496. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487
    1. Krauthamer Ewing ES, Herres J, Dilks KE, Rahim F, Trentacosta CJ. Understanding of emotions and empathy: predictors of positive parenting with preschoolers in economically stressed families. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(5):1346–1358. doi:10.1007/s10826-018-01303-6
    1. Queiroz P, Garcia OF, Garcia F, Zacares JJ, Camino C. Self and nature: parental socialization, self-esteem, and environmental values in Spanish adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3732. doi:10.3390/ijerph17103732
    1. Fernando G, Enrique G. Is always authoritative the optimum parenting style? Evidence from Spanish families. Adolescence. 2009;44(173):101–131.
    1. Longtin S, Gerber S. Contemporary perspectives on facilitating language acquisition for children on the autistic spectrum: engaging the parent and the child. J Dev Process. 2008;3(1):38–51.
    1. Patterson SY, Smith V. The experience of parents of toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder in the more than words parent education program. Infants Young Child. 2011;24(4):329–343. doi:10.1097/IYC.0b013e31822c10e4
    1. Krupa M, Boominathan P, Sebastian S, Venkat Ramanan P. Child-directed communication behaviours during mother-child interaction in children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children in south India. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2019;67(2018):101423. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101423
    1. Cochet H, Byrne RW. Communication in the second and third year of life: relationships between nonverbal social skills and language. Infant Behav Dev. 2016;44:189–198. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.07.003
    1. Harbison AL, McDaniel J, Yoder PJ. The association of imperative and declarative intentional communication with language in young children with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. In: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 36. Elsevier Ltd; 2017:21–34.
    1. Camaioni L, Perucchini P, Bellagamba F, Colonnesi C. The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy. 2004;5(3):291–308. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0503_3
    1. Dunham P, Dunham F. Optimal social structures and adaptive infant development; 1995. Available from: . Accessed June23, 2021.
    1. Bruner J. Play, thought, and language. Peabody J Educ. 1983;60(3):60–69. doi:10.1080/01619568309538407
    1. Mirenda PL, Donnellan AM, Yoder DE. Gaze behavior: a new look at an old problem. J Autism Dev Disord. 1983;13(4):397–409. doi:10.1007/BF01531588
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Baldwin DA, Crowson M. Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Dev. 1997;68(1):48–57. doi:10.2307/1131924
    1. Bretherton I. The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Dev Psychol. 1992;28(5):759–775. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759
    1. Lee K, Schertz HH. Brief report: analysis of the relationship between turn taking and joint attention for toddlers with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50(7):2633–2640. doi:10.1007/s10803-019-03979-1
    1. Smilansky S. The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool Children. 605 Third Avenue, New York, New York 10016 ($7.50): John Wiley and Sons, Inc.; 1968.
    1. Pellegrini AD, Bjorklund DF. The ontogeny and phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Human Nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.). 2004;15(1):23–43. doi:10.1007/s12110-004-1002-z
    1. Whitebread D, Neale D, Solis L. The role of play in children’s development: a review of the evidence; 2018. Available from: . Accessed June23, 2021.
    1. Smith LB, Jones SS. Symbolic play connects to language through visual object recognition. Dev Sci. 2011;14(5):1142–1149. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01065.x
    1. McCune L. A normative study of representational play at the transition to language. Dev Psychol. 1995;31(2):198–206. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.2.198
    1. Casby MW, Della CM. Symbolic play performance and early language development. J Psycholinguist Res. 1987;16(1):31–42. doi:10.1007/BF01067749
    1. Veneziano E. Early language and nonverbal representation: a reassessment. J Child Lang. 1981;8(3):541–563. doi:10.1017/S0305000900003421
    1. Lillard AS, Lerner MD, Hopkins EJ, Dore RA, Smith ED, Palmquist CM. The impact of pretend play on children’s development: a review of the evidence. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(1):1–34. doi:10.1037/a0029321
    1. Han M, Moore N, Vukelich C, Buell M. Does play make a difference? How play intervention affects the vocabulary learning of at-risk preschoolers. - PsycNET. Am J Play. 2010;3(1):82–105.
    1. Crais ER, Watson LR, Baranek GT. Use of gesture development in profiling children’s prelinguistic communication skills. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2009;18(1):95–108. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2008/07-0041)
    1. Lytton H. Observation studies of parent-child interaction: a methodological review. Child Dev. 1971;42(3):651. doi:10.2307/1127439
    1. Sommer A, Hachul C, Roßbach HG. Video-based assessment and rating of parent-child interaction within the national educational panel study. Methodol Issues Longitud Surv Ex Natl Educ Panel Stud. 2016;151–168.
    1. Tait DM. Video analysis: a method of assessing changes in preverbal and early linguistic communication after cochlear implantation. Ear Heart. 1993;14(6):378–389. doi:10.1097/00003446-199312000-00002
    1. Tait ME, Nikolopoulos TP, Lutman ME, Wilson D, Wells P. Video analysis of pre-verbal communication behaviours: use and reliability. Deaf Educ Int. 2001;3(1):38–43. doi:10.1179/146431501790561106
    1. Howe D. Disabled children, parent-child interaction and attachment. Child Fam Soc Work. 2006;11(2):95–106. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2206.2006.00397.x
    1. Marfo K, Dedrick C, Barbour N. Mother-child interactions and the development of children with mental retardation; 1998. Available from: . Accessed June23, 2021.
    1. McCollum J, Hemmeter M. Parent-child interaction intervention when children have disabilities. Eff Early Interv. 1997;549–576.
    1. Landa RJ, Holman KC, O’Neill AH, Stuart EA. Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(1):13–21. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x
    1. Lakkanna S, Venkatesh K, Bhat JS. Assessment of language development. Bak CA Omni Ther Serv. 2008.
    1. Tiwari S, Kumar A, Kumar A. Development & standardization of a scale to measure socio-economic status in urban & rural communities in India. Indian J Med Res. 2005;122(4):309.
    1. Muentener P, Herrig E, Schulz L. The efficiency of infants’ exploratory play is related to longer-term cognitive development. Front Psychol. 2018;9(MAY):1–18. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00635
    1. Fenson L, Schell RE. The origins of exploratory play. Early Child Dev Care. 1985;19(1–2):3–24. doi:10.1080/0300443850190102
    1. Williams E, Reddy V, Costall A. Taking a closer look at functional play in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):67–77. doi:10.1023/A:1005665714197
    1. Pepper J, Weitzman E. It Takes Two to Talk: A Practical Guide for Parents of Children with … - Jan Pepper, Elaine Weitzman, Hanen Centre - Google Books. 4th ed. Hanen Centre Publication; 2004.
    1. Romeo RR, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, et al. Beyond the 30-million-word gap: children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol Sci. 2018;29(5):700–710. doi:10.1177/0956797617742725
    1. Bortoli A, Brown PM. Assessing prelinguistic communication in children with developmental delay: effects of contextual structure. Australas J Spec Educ. 2000;24(2–3):74–84. doi:10.1017/S1030011200024751
    1. Van Balkom H, Verhoeven L, Van Weerdenburg M. Conversational behaviour of children with developmental language delay and their caretakers. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2010;45(3):295–319. doi:10.3109/13682820902994226
    1. Rescorla L, Bascome A, Lampard J, Feeny N. Conversational patterns in late talkers at age 3. Appl Psycholinguist. 2001;22(2):235–251. doi:10.1017/S0142716401002053
    1. Ellis Weismer S, Davidson MM, Gangopadhyay I, Sindberg H, Roebuck H, Kaushanskaya M. The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9(1). doi:10.1186/s11689-017-9209-6
    1. Bishop DVM, Chan J, Adams C, Hartley J, Weir F. Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12(2):177–199. doi:10.1017/S0954579400002042
    1. Stockman IJ. The new peabody picture vocabulary test-III: an illusion of unbiased assessment? Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2000;31(4):340–353. doi:10.1044/0161-1461.3104.340
    1. Arens K, Cress C, Marvin C. Gaze-shift patterns of young children with developmental disabilities who are at risk for being nonspeaking. Spec Educ Commun Disord Fac Publ. 2005.
    1. Wadnerkar Kamble M, Lam-Cassettari C, James DM. Communication skills and communicative autonomy of prelinguistic deaf and hard-of-hearing children: application of a video feedback intervention. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1983. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01983
    1. Paul R, Looney SS, Dahm PS. Communication and socialization skills at ages 2 and 3 in “late-talking” young children. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(4):858–865. doi:10.1044/jshr.3404.858
    1. Martínez I, Murgui S, Garcia OF, Garcia F. Parenting and adolescent adjustment: the mediational role of family self-esteem. J Child Fam Stud. 2021;30(5):1184–1197. doi:10.1007/s10826-021-01937-z
    1. Perez-Gramaje AF, Garcia OF, Reyes M, Serra E, Garcia F. Parenting styles and aggressive adolescents: relationships with self-esteem and personal maladjustment. Eur J Psychol Appl Leg Context. 2020;12(1):1–10.
    1. DeVeney S, Cress CJ, Lambert M. Parental directiveness and responsivity toward young children with complex communication needs. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2016;18(1):53–64. doi:10.3109/17549507.2015.1081282
    1. Hoffer PC, Bliss LS. Maternal verbal responsiveness with language-impaired, stage-matched, and age-matched normal children. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1990;11(3):305–319. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(90)90012-9
    1. Iacono T, Carter M, Hook J. Identification of intentional communication in students with severe and multiple disabilities. AAC Augment Altern Commun. 1998;14(2):102–114. doi:10.1080/07434619812331278246
    1. Cress CJ, Grabast J, Burgers Jerke K. Contingent interactions between parents and young children with severe expressive communication impairments. Commun Disord Q. 2013;34(2):81–96. doi:10.1177/1525740111416644
    1. Village G. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child. 2007.
    1. Casby M. Symbolic play II: a unified model of symbolic play. Infant Toddler Interv. 1991;1:233–243.
    1. Smith PK. Play: types and functions in human development. In: Ellis BJ, Bjorklund DF, editors. Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. Guilford Press; 2005:271–291.
    1. Nicolopoulou A, Barbosa De Sá A, Ilgaz H, Brockmeyer C. Using the transformative power of play to educate hearts and minds: from Vygotsky to Vivian Paley and beyond. Mind Cult Act. 2010;17(1):42–58. doi:10.1080/10749030903312512
    1. Meins E, Fernyhough C, Arnott B, Leekam SR, de Rosnay M. Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Dev. 2013;84(5):1777–1790. doi:10.1111/cdev.12061
    1. Graham KL, Burghardt GM. Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. Q Rev Biol. 2010;85(4):393–418. doi:10.1086/656903
    1. Barton EE. Teaching generalized pretend play and related behaviors to young children with disabilities. Except Child. 2015;81(4):489–506. doi:10.1177/0014402914563694
    1. Thiemann-Bourque KS, Brady NC, Fleming KK. Symbolic play of preschoolers with severe communication impairments with autism and other developmental delays: more similarities than differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(5):863–873. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1317-7
    1. Reichow B, Boyd BA, Barton EE, Odom SL. Handbook of Early Childhood Special Education. Springer International Publishing; 2016:1–594 p.
    1. Westby CE. A scale for assessing development of children’s play. Play diagnosis and assessment. Play Diagn Assess. 2000;2:15–57.
    1. Spiker D, Boyce GC, Boyce LK. Parent-child interactions when young children have disabilities. Int Rev Res Ment Retard. 2002;25:35–70.
    1. Warren SF, Brady NC. The role of maternal responsivity in the development of children with intellectual disabilities. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(4):330–338. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20177

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren