Breaking up prolonged sitting with moderate-intensity walking improves attention and executive function in Qatari females

Bryna C R Chrismas, Lee Taylor, Anissa Cherif, Suzan Sayegh, Daniel P Bailey, Bryna C R Chrismas, Lee Taylor, Anissa Cherif, Suzan Sayegh, Daniel P Bailey

Abstract

Background: Cultural, environmental and logistical factors promote a sedentary lifestyle within Qatar, particularly for females. Sedentary behaviour is acutely associated with poor cognitive function and fatigue, and chronically may be implicated with cognitive decline (i.e. Alzheimer's disease).

Purpose: To examine the effects of breaking up sitting with short-duration frequent walking bouts on cognitive function and fatigue in Qatari females.

Method: Eleven sedentary (sitting ≥7 h/day) females completed three visits; the first being familiarisation. In a cross-over randomised manner, experimental visits two and three were identical, except participants either remained seated for 5-h (SIT) or interrupted their sitting every 30-min with a 3-min moderate-intensity walk (WALK) on a motorised treadmill. The Computerised Mental Performance Assessment System (COMPASS) assessed cognition at baseline (-15-min), and then at 2.5-h and 5-h into the experimental conditions. Specific COMPASS tasks employed were; serial-3 subtractions (2-min), serial-7 subtractions (2-min), simple reaction time (RT; 50 stimuli), rapid visual information processing [RVIP (5-min)], choice reaction time (CRT; 50 stimuli), and Stroop (60 stimuli); and a visual analogue scale for fatigue (VAS-F) was completed at the same time intervals.

Results: There was a significant condition effect for CRT (f = 26.7, p = 0.007). On average CRT was 101 s (95% CI = -47 to -156 s) quicker in WALK compared to SIT. There was a significant time effect for CRT (f = 15.5, p = 0.01). On average CRT was 134 s slower at 5-h compared to baseline (p = 0.006; 95% CI = -64 to -203 s), and 114 s slower at 5-h compared to 2.5-h (p = 0.01; 95% CI = -44 to -183 s). There was a significant interaction effect for RT in the Stroop incongruent task (f = 10.0, p = 0.03). On average RT was 210 s quicker at 2.5-h in WALK compared to SIT (p = 0.01; 95% CI = -76 to -346 s).

Conclusion: Breaking up prolonged sitting with moderate-intensity walking offers an ecologically valid intervention to enhance some aspects of cognitive function, whilst not affecting fatigue in sedentary Qatari females. Whilst these findings are promising, the long-term effects of breaking up sitting on cognitive function requires testing before population level recommendations can be made.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Experimental schematic.
Fig 1. Experimental schematic.
SIT = uninterrupted sitting; WALK = breaking up sitting with walking. Both conditions are identical except in SIT, no walking breaks occurred.

References

    1. Qatar Biobank. Qatar Biobank report Qatar2016-2017. Available from: .
    1. Sayegh S, Van Der Walt M, Al-Kuwari MG. One-year assessment of physical activity level in adult Qatari females: a pedometer-based longitudinal study. International journal of women's health. 2016;8:287–93. 10.2147/IJWH.S99943 .
    1. Klautzer L, Becker J, Mattke S. The curse of wealth—Middle Eastern countries need to address the rapidly rising burden of diabetes. International journal of health policy and management. 2014;2(3):109–14. 10.15171/ijhpm.2014.33 .
    1. Magnon V, Vallet GT, Auxiette C. Sedentary Behavior at Work and Cognitive Functioning: A Systematic Review. Frontiers in public health. 2018;6:239–. 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00239 .
    1. Lovden M, Xu W, Wang HX. Lifestyle change and the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia: what is the evidence? Current opinion in psychiatry. 2013;26(3):239–43. Epub 2013/03/16. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32835f4135 .
    1. Wheeler MJ, Dempsey PC, Grace MS, Ellis KA, Gardiner PA, Green DJ, et al. Sedentary behavior as a risk factor for cognitive decline? A focus on the influence of glycemic control in brain health. Alzheimer's & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions. 2017;3(3):291–300. 10.1016/j.trci.2017.04.001
    1. Carter SE, Draijer R, Holder SM, Brown L, Thijssen DHJ, Hopkins ND. Regular walking breaks prevent the decline in cerebral blood flow associated with prolonged sitting. Journal of applied physiology (Bethesda, Md: 1985). 2018;125(3):790–8. Epub 2018/06/08. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00310.2018 .
    1. Gibson AM, Muggeridge DJ, Hughes AR, Kelly L, Kirk A. An examination of objectively-measured sedentary behavior and mental well-being in adults across week days and weekends. PloS one. 2017;12(9):e0185143 Epub 2017/09/22. 10.1371/journal.pone.0185143
    1. Qatar National Physical Activity Guidelines. Qatar National Physical Activity Guidelines Qatar: Aspetar Orthopaedic & Sports Medicine Hospital; 2014. First:[72]. Available from: .
    1. Karakolis T, Callaghan JP. The impact of sit-stand office workstations on worker discomfort and productivity: a review. Applied ergonomics. 2014;45(3):799–806. Epub 2013/10/26. 10.1016/j.apergo.2013.10.001 .
    1. Buckley JP, Hedge A, Yates T, Copeland RJ, Loosemore M, Hamer M, et al. The sedentary office: an expert statement on the growing case for change towards better health and productivity. British journal of sports medicine. 2015;49(21):1357–62. Epub 2015/06/03. 10.1136/bjsports-2015-094618 .
    1. Ojo SO, Bailey DP, Chater AM, Hewson DJ. The Impact of Active Workstations on Workplace Productivity and Performance: A Systematic Review. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2018;15(3). Epub 2018/03/03. 10.3390/ijerph15030417
    1. Baker R, Coenen P, Howie E, Lee J, Williamson A, Straker L. A detailed description of the short-term musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of prolonged standing for office computer work. Ergonomics. 2018;61(7):877–90. Epub 2018/02/02. 10.1080/00140139.2017.1420825 .
    1. Katzmarzyk PT, Church TS, Craig CL, Bouchard C. Sitting time and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2009;41(5):998–1005. Epub 2009/04/07. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181930355 .
    1. Bergouignan A, Legget KT, De Jong N, Kealey E, Nikolovski J, Groppel JL, et al. Effect of frequent interruptions of prolonged sitting on self-perceived levels of energy, mood, food cravings and cognitive function. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2016;13(1):113 Epub 2016/11/05. 10.1186/s12966-016-0437-z
    1. Bailey DP, Locke CD. Breaking up prolonged sitting with light-intensity walking improves postprandial glycemia, but breaking up sitting with standing does not. Journal of science and medicine in sport. 2015;18(3):294–8. Epub 2014/04/08. 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.03.008 .
    1. Bailey DP, Broom DR, Chrismas BC, Taylor L, Flynn E, Hough J. Breaking up prolonged sitting time with walking does not affect appetite or gut hormone concentrations but does induce an energy deficit and suppresses postprandial glycaemia in sedentary adults. Applied physiology, nutrition, and metabolism = Physiologie appliquee, nutrition et metabolisme. 2016;41(3):324–31. Epub 2016/02/13. 10.1139/apnm-2015-0462 .
    1. Sundström Poromaa I, Gingnell M. Menstrual cycle influence on cognitive function and emotion processing-from a reproductive perspective. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2014;8:380-. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00008 .
    1. Chan AW, Tetzlaff JM, Altman DG, Laupacis A, Gotzsche PC, Krle AJK, et al. SPIRIT 2013 Statement: defining standard protocol items for clinical trials. Revista panamericana de salud publica = Pan American journal of public health. 2015;38(6):506–14. Epub 2016/07/22.
    1. Borg GA. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1982;14(5):377–81. Epub 1982/01/01. .
    1. Lee KA, Hicks G, Nino-Murcia G. Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry research. 1991;36(3):291–8. Epub 1991/03/01. .
    1. Mifflin MD, St Jeor ST, Hill LA, Scott BJ, Daugherty SA, Koh YO. A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 1990;51(2):241–7. Epub 1990/02/01. 10.1093/ajcn/51.2.241 .
    1. Bailey DP, Maylor BD, Orton CJ, Zakrzewski-Fruer JK. Effects of breaking up prolonged sitting following low and high glycaemic index breakfast consumption on glucose and insulin concentrations. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2017;117(7):1299–307. 10.1007/s00421-017-3610-4
    1. Kennedy DO, Wightman EL, Forster J, Khan J, Haskell-Ramsay CF, Jackson PA. Cognitive and Mood Effects of a Nutrient Enriched Breakfast Bar in Healthy Adults: A Randomised, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Parallel Groups Study. Nutrients. 2017;9(12):1332 10.3390/nu9121332 .
    1. Scholey AB, French SJ, Morris PJ, Kennedy DO, Milne AL, Haskell CF. Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. Journal of psychopharmacology (Oxford, England). 2010;24(10):1505–14. Epub 2009/11/28. 10.1177/0269881109106923 .
    1. Reay JL, Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. Single doses of Panax ginseng (G115) reduce blood glucose levels and improve cognitive performance during sustained mental activity. Journal of psychopharmacology (Oxford, England). 2005;19(4):357–65. Epub 2005/06/29. 10.1177/0269881105053286 .
    1. Haskell-Ramsay CF, Jackson PA, Dodd FL, Forster JS, Berube J, Levinton C, et al. Acute Post-Prandial Cognitive Effects of Brown Seaweed Extract in Humans. Nutrients. 2018;10(1). Epub 2018/01/19. 10.3390/nu10010085
    1. Grafen G, Hails R. Modern statistics for the life sciences. New York, USA: Oxford University Press; 2002.
    1. West BT, Welch KB, Galecki AT. Linear Mixed Models: A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software. 2nd ed: Taylor and Francis; 2014.
    1. Hurvich CM, Tsai CL. Model selection for extended quasi-likelihood models in small samples. Biometrics. 1995;51(3):1077–84. Epub 1995/09/01. .
    1. Hommel G. A stagewise rejective multiple test procedure based on a modified Bonferroni test. Biometrika. 1988;75(2):383–6. 10.1093/biomet/75.2.383
    1. Bergouignan A, Latouche C, Heywood S, Grace MS, Reddy-Luthmoodoo M, Natoli AK, et al. Frequent interruptions of sedentary time modulates contraction- and insulin-stimulated glucose uptake pathways in muscle: Ancillary analysis from randomized clinical trials. Scientific reports. 2016;6:32044 Epub 2016/08/25. 10.1038/srep32044
    1. Vincent GE, Jay SM, Sargent C, Kovac K, Vandelanotte C, Ridgers ND, et al. The impact of breaking up prolonged sitting on glucose metabolism and cognitive function when sleep is restricted. Neurobiology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms. 2018;4:17–23. 10.1016/j.nbscr.2017.09.001. 10.1016/j.nbscr.2017.09.001
    1. Labonté-LeMoyne É, Santhanam R, Léger P-M, Courtemanche F, Fredette M, Sénécal S. The delayed effect of treadmill desk usage on recall and attention. Computers in Human Behavior. 2015;46:1–5. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.054.
    1. Sperlich B, De Clerck I, Zinner C, Holmberg H-C, Wallmann-Sperlich B. Prolonged Sitting Interrupted by 6-Min of High-Intensity Exercise: Circulatory, Metabolic, Hormonal, Thermal, Cognitive, and Perceptual Responses. Frontiers in Physiology. 2018;9(1279). 10.3389/fphys.2018.01279
    1. Larson MJ, LeCheminant JD, Hill K, Carbine K, Masterson T, Christenson E. Cognitive and typing outcomes measured simultaneously with slow treadmill walking or sitting: implications for treadmill desks. PloS one. 2015;10(4):e0121309 Epub 2015/04/16. 10.1371/journal.pone.0121309
    1. Kennedy DO, Wightman EL, Forster J, Khan J, Haskell-Ramsay CF, Jackson PA. Cognitive and Mood Effects of a Nutrient Enriched Breakfast Bar in Healthy Adults: A Randomised, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Parallel Groups Study. Nutrients. 2017;9(12). Epub 2017/12/08. 10.3390/nu9121332
    1. Hoyland A, Lawton CL, Dye L. Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: a systematic research review. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2008;32(1):72–85. Epub 2007/07/17. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.006 .
    1. Veasey RC, Haskell-Ramsay CF, Kennedy DO, Tiplady B, Stevenson EJ. The Effect of Breakfast Prior to Morning Exercise on Cognitive Performance, Mood and Appetite Later in the Day in Habitually Active Women. Nutrients. 2015;7(7):5712–32. Epub 2015/07/18. 10.3390/nu7075250
    1. Wennberg P, Boraxbekk C-J, Wheeler M, Howard B, Dempsey PC, Lambert G, et al. Acute effects of breaking up prolonged sitting on fatigue and cognition: a pilot study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(2). 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009630
    1. Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. Glucose administration, heart rate and cognitive performance: effects of increasing mental effort. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149(1):63–71. Epub 2000/05/02. .

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren