Stroboscopic vision and sustained attention during coincidence-anticipation

Rafael Ballester, Florentino Huertas, Makoto Uji, Simon J Bennett, Rafael Ballester, Florentino Huertas, Makoto Uji, Simon J Bennett

Abstract

We compared coincidence-anticipation performance in normal vision and stroboscopic vision as a function of time-on-task. Participants estimated the arrival time of a real object that moved with constant acceleration (-0.7, 0, +0.7 m/s2) in a pseudo-randomised order across 4 blocks of 30 trials in both vision conditions, received in a counter-balanced order. Participants (n = 20) became more errorful (accuracy and variability) in the normal vision condition as a function of time-on-task, whereas performance was maintained in the stroboscopic vision condition. We interpret these data as showing that participants failed to maintain coincidence-anticipation performance in the normal vision condition due to monotony and attentional underload. In contrast, the stroboscopic vision condition placed a greater demand on visual-spatial memory for motion extrapolation, and thus participants did not experience the typical vigilance decrement in performance. While short-term adaptation effects from practicing in stroboscopic vision are promising, future work needs to consider for how long participants can maintain effortful processing, and whether there are negative carry-over effects from cognitive fatigue when transferring to normal vision.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Temporal course of the stimuli presentation in the PVT.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Group mean constant error as a function of time-on-task (Block 1–4) for the normal vision and stroboscopic vision conditions. Vertical bars represent standard errors of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Group mean variable error as a function of time-on-task (Block 1–4) for the normal vision and stroboscopic vision conditions. Vertical bars represent standard errors of the mean.

References

    1. Elliott D, Chua R, Pollock BJ. The influence of intermittent vision on manual aiming. Acta Psychol. 1994;85:1–13. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90016-7.
    1. Mitroff SR, Friesen P, Bennett D, Yoo H, Reichow A. Enhancing ice hockey skills through stroboscopic visual training–A pilot study. Athl Train Sports Health Care. 2013;5:261–264. doi: 10.3928/19425864-20131030-02.
    1. Clark JF, Ellis JK, Bench J, Khoury J, Graman P. High-performance vision training improves batting statistics for University of Cincinnati baseball players. PloS One. 2012;7:e29109. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029109.
    1. Appelbaum, L. G. & Erickson, G. Sports vision training: A review of the state-of-the-art in digital training techniques. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol, 1–30 (2016).
    1. Appelbaum LG, Cain MS, Schroeder JE, Darling EF, Mitroff SR. Stroboscopic visual training improves information encoding in short-term memory. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2012;74:1681–1691. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0344-6.
    1. Smith TQ, Mitroff SR. Stroboscopic training enhances anticipatory timing. Int J Exerc Sci. 2012;5:344–353.
    1. Appelbaum LG, Schroeder JE, Cain MS, Darling EF, Mitroff SR. Improved visual cognition through stroboscopic training. Front. Psychol. 2011;2:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00276.
    1. Athletic Republic. Sports Training Origin of Nike Vapor Strobe. Available at (2011).
    1. Flombaum JI, Scholl BJ, Pylyshyn ZW. Attentional resources in visual tracking through occlusion: The high-beams effect. Cognition. 2008;107:904–931. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.015.
    1. Warm JS, Parasuraman R, Matthews G. Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum Factors. 2008;50:433–441. doi: 10.1518/001872008X312152.
    1. Ripoll H, Latiri I. Effect of expertise on coincident-timing accuracy in a fast ball game. J Sports Sci. 1997;15:573–580. doi: 10.1080/026404197367001.
    1. Gunzelmann, G., Moore, L. R., Gluck, K. A., Van Dongen, H. P. A. & Dinges, D.F. Fatigue in sustained attention: Generalizing mechanisms for time awake to time on task in Cognitive Fatigue: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Current Research and Future Applications (ed. Ackerman, P.L.) 83–96 (American Psychological Association, 2010).
    1. Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E. Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiol Behav. 2008;93:369–378. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.09.016.
    1. Schlyter, P. Radiometry and photometry in astronomy. Available at: (2015).
    1. Drummond SPA, et al. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep. 2005;28:1059–1068.
    1. Loh S, Lamond N, Dorrian J, Roach G, Dawson D. The validity of psychomotor vigilance tasks of less than 10-minute duration. Behav Res Methods, Instrum, & Comput. 2004;36:339–346. doi: 10.3758/BF03195580.
    1. Ballester R, Huertas F, Yuste FJ, Llorens F, Sanabria D. The relationship between regular sports participation and vigilance in male and female adolescents. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0123898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123898.
    1. Wilson A, Dollman J, Lushington K, Olds T. Reliability of the 5-min psychomotor vigilance task in a primary school classroom setting. Behav Res Methods. 2010;42:754–758. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.754.
    1. Ballester, R., Huertas, F., Molina, E. & Sanabria, D. Sport participation and vigilance in children: Influence of different sport expertise. Journal of Sport and Health Science In press; 10.1016/j.jshs.2017.05.008 (2017).
    1. Wilkinson RT, Houghton D. Field test of arousal: A portable reaction timer with data storage. Hum Factors. 1982;24:487–493. doi: 10.1177/001872088202400409.
    1. Appelbaum LG, Lu Y, Khanna R, Detwiler KR. The Effects of Sports Vision Training on Sensorimotor Abilities in Collegiate Softball Athletes. Athl Train Sports Health Care. 2016;8:154–163. doi: 10.3928/19425864-20160314-01.
    1. De Valois RL, Morgan H, Snodderly DM. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision-III. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. Vis Res. 1974;14:75–81. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90118-7.
    1. Billino J, Bremmer F, Gegenfurtner KR. Motion processing at low light levels: Differential effects on the perception of specific motion types. J Vis. 2008;8:1–10. doi: 10.1167/8.3.14.
    1. Lencer R, et al. Cortical mechanisms of smooth pursuit eye movements with object blanking: An fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:1430–1436. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03229.x.
    1. Wilkins L, Gray R. Effects of stroboscopic visual training on visual attention, motion perception, and catching performance. Percept Mot Skills. 2015;121:57–79. doi: 10.2466/22.25.PMS.121c11x0.
    1. Oken BS, Salinsky MC, Elsas SM. Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1885–1901. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017.
    1. Grooms D, Appelbaum G, Onate J. Neuroplasticity following anterior cruciate ligament injury: a framework for visual-motor training approaches in rehabilitation. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2015;45:381–393. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2015.5549.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren