Executive functions

Adele Diamond, Adele Diamond

Abstract

Executive functions (EFs) make possible mentally playing with ideas; taking the time to think before acting; meeting novel, unanticipated challenges; resisting temptations; and staying focused. Core EFs are inhibition [response inhibition (self-control--resisting temptations and resisting acting impulsively) and interference control (selective attention and cognitive inhibition)], working memory, and cognitive flexibility (including creatively thinking "outside the box," seeing anything from different perspectives, and quickly and flexibly adapting to changed circumstances). The developmental progression and representative measures of each are discussed. Controversies are addressed (e.g., the relation between EFs and fluid intelligence, self-regulation, executive attention, and effortful control, and the relation between working memory and inhibition and attention). The importance of social, emotional, and physical health for cognitive health is discussed because stress, lack of sleep, loneliness, or lack of exercise each impair EFs. That EFs are trainable and can be improved with practice is addressed, including diverse methods tried thus far.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Passive-dissipation model showing how delay can improve performance on inhibitory tasks (from Simpson et al. 2011).
Figure 2
Figure 2
At every age studied, children were slower and less accurate on the congruent block than on the incongruent block. That effect is completely absent in adults, who are as fast and as accurate on the incongruent block as on the congruent one. The memory demands of those two blocks were the same; they differ only in that the incongruent block requires inhibitory control and the congruent block does not (based on Davidson et al. 2006; this is now called Hearts and Flowers.).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Comparison of the mixed conditions of the Dots (now called Hearts and Flowers) and Simon tasks in percentage of correct responses (based on Davidson et al. 2006).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Executive functions and related terms.

References

    1. Alain C, Woods DL. Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visual attention: evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory. Psychol. Aging. 1999;14:507–19.
    1. Alloway TP. Working memory, reading and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2007;96:20–36.
    1. Alloway TP, Gathercole SE, Kirkwood H, Elliott J. The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Dev. 2009;80:606–21.
    1. Alloway TP, Gathercole SE, Willis C, Adams A-M. A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2004;87:85–106.
    1. Allport A, Wylie G. Task switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In: Monsell S, Driver J, editors. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVII. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2000. pp. 35–70.
    1. Anderson MC, Levy B. Suppressing unwanted memories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009;18:189–94.
    1. Arnsten AFT. The biology of being frazzled. Science. 1998;280:1711–12.
    1. Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:e55–68.
    1. Awh E, Anllo-Vento L, Hillyard SA. The role of spatial selective attention in working memory for locations: evidence from event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000;12:840–47.
    1. Awh E, Jonides J. Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001;5:119–26.
    1. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology. 1994;8:485–93.
    1. Bailey CE. Cognitive accuracy and intelligent executive function in the brain and in business. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2007;1118:122–41.
    1. Baldo JV, Shimamura AP. Letter and category fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology. 1997;12:259–67.
    1. Baldo JV, Shimamura AP, Delis DC, Kramer J, Kaplan E. Verbal and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2001;7:586–96.
    1. Baler RD, Volkow ND. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends Mol. Med. 2006;12:559–66.
    1. Band GP, van der Molen MW, Logan GD. Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychol. (Amsterdam) 2003;112:105–42.
    1. Barch DM. The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2005;1:321–53.
    1. Barnes ME, Gozal D, Molfese DL. Attention in children with obstructive sleep apnoea: an event-related potentials study. Sleep Med. 2012;13:368–77.
    1. Barr RA, Giambra LM. Age-related decrement in auditory selective attention. Psychol. Aging. 1990;5:597–99.
    1. Barrouillet P, Gavens N, Vergauwe E, Gaillard V, Camos V. Working memory span development: a time-based resource-sharing model account. Dev. Psychol. 2009;45:477–90.
    1. Baumeister RF, Twenge JM, Nuss CK. Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002;83:817–27.
    1. Bell MA, Cuevas K. Psychobiology of executive function in early development. In: McCardle P, Freund L, Griffin JA, editors. Executive Function in Preschool Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment and Translational Research. Am. Psychol. Assoc.; Washington, DC: 2012. In press.
    1. Bergman Nutley S, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Thorell LB, Humphreys K, Klingberg T. Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:591–601.
    1. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 2010;30:331–551.
    1. Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Klein R, Mythili V. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychol. Aging. 2004;19:290–303.
    1. Bialystok E, Viswanathan M. Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition. 2009;112:494–500.
    1. Blair C, Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008;20:899–911.
    1. Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false-belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78:647–63.
    1. Bodrova E, Leong DJ. Tools of the Mind: The Vygotskian Approach to Early Childhood education. Merrill/Prentice Hall; New York: 2007.
    1. Borella E, Carretti B, Pelgrina S. The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010;43:541–52.
    1. Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Brame B, Dodge KA, Fergusson DE. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: a six-site cross-national study. Dev. Psychol. 2003;30:222–45.
    1. Brooks P, Hanauer JB, Padowska B, Rosman H. The role of selective attention in preschoolers’ rule use in a novel dimensional card sort. Cogn. Dev. 2003;117:1–21.
    1. Brown TE, Landgraf JM. Improvements in executive function correlate with enhanced performance and functioning and health-related quality of life: evidence from 2 large, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trials in ADHD. Postgrad. Med. 2010;122:42–51.
    1. Bruner JS, Olver RR, Greenfield PM. Studies in Cognitive Growth: A Collaboration at the Center for Cognitive Studies. Wiley; New York: 1966.
    1. Bunge SA, Dudukovic NM, Thomason ME, Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JDE. Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fMRI. Neuron. 2002;33:301–11.
    1. Burgess PW, Simons JS. Theories of frontal lobe executive function: clinical applications. In: Halligan PW, Wade DT, editors. Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 2005. pp. 211–31.
    1. Cacioppo J, Patrick W. Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. Norton; New York: 2008. Summarizes pioneering research on startling and profound effects of loneliness.

    1. Campbell WK, Krusemark EA, Dyckman KA, Brunell AB, McDowell JE, et al. A magnetoencephalography investigation of neural correlates for social exclusion and self-control. Soc. Neurosci. 2006;1:124–34.
    1. Carr LA, Nigg JT, Henderson JM. Attentional versus motor inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:430–41.
    1. Case R. Capacity-based explanations of working memory growth: a brief history and reevaluation. In: Weinert FE, Schneider W, editors. Memory Performance and Competencies: Issues in Growth and Development. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1995. pp. 23–44.
    1. Case R, Kurland DM, Goldberg J. Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1982;33:386–404.
    1. Casey BJ, Siegler RS, McClelland JL. Development and disruption of inhibitory mechanisms of attention. Mechanisms of Cognitive Development: The Carnegie Symposium on Cognition; Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. pp. 327–49.
    1. Cepeda NJ, Kramer AF, Gonzalez de Sather JC. Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Dev. Psychol. 2001;37:715–30.
    1. Cepeda NJ, Munakata Y. Why do children perseverate when they seem to know better: graded working memory, or directed inhibition? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2007;14:1058–65.
    1. Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Cohen NJ. Aerobic fitness and executive control of relational memory in preadolescent children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2011;43:344–49.
    1. Chatham CH, Yerys BE, Munakata Y. Why won’t you do what I want? The informative failures of children and models. Cogn. Dev. 2012 In press.
    1. Chein JM, Schneider W. Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005;25:607–23.
    1. Chi YK, Kim TH, Han JW, Lee SB, Park JH, et al. Impaired design fluency is a marker of pathological cognitive aging: results from the Korean longitudinal study on health and aging. Psychiatry Invest. 2012;9:59–64.
    1. Cohen JR, Berkman ET, Lieberman MD. Ventrolateral PFC as a self-control muscle and how to use it without trying. Oxford Handb. Frontal Lobe Funct. 2012 In press.
    1. Cohen S, Bixenman M, Meiran N, Diamond A. Task switching in children. Presented at S. Carolina Bicentenn. Symp. Attention; Columbia: Univ. S. Carolina; 2001.
    1. Collins A, Koechlin E. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biol. 2012;10:e1001293.
    1. Collins P, Roberts AC, Dias R, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Perseveration and strategy in a novel spatial self-ordered task for nonhuman primates: effect of excitotoxic lesions and dopamine depletions of the prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1998;10:332–54.
    1. Conway ARA, Engle RW. Working memory and retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 1994;123:354–73.
    1. Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Bunting MF, Hambrick DZ, Wilhelm O, Engle RW. Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2005;12:769–86.
    1. Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:547–52.
    1. Cowan N, AuBuchon AM, Gilchrist AL, Ricker TJ, Saults JS. Age differences in visual working memory capacity: not based on encoding limitations. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:1066–74.
    1. Cowan N, Saults JS, Elliot EM. The search for what is fundamental in the development of working memory. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2002;29:1–49.
    1. Cragg L, Nation K. Go or no-go? Developmental improvements in the efficiency of response inhibition in mid-childhood. Dev. Sci. 2008;11:819–27.
    1. Crescioni AW, Ehrlinger J, Alquist JL, Conlon KE, Baumeister RF, et al. High trait self-control predicts positive health behaviors and success in weight loss. J. Health Psychol. 2011;16:750–59.
    1. Crone EA, Wendelken C, Donohue SE, van Leijenhorst L, Bunge SA. Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:9315–20.
    1. Czernochowski D, Nessler D, Friedman D. On why not to rush older adults—relying on reactive cognitive control can effectively reduce errors at the expense of slowed responses. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:637–46.
    1. Daneman M, Carpenter P. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980;19:450–66.
    1. Darowski ES, Helder E, Zacks RT, Hasher L, Hambrick DZ. Age-related differences in cognition: the role of distraction control. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:638–44.
    1. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4–13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2037–78.
    1. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, et al. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2011;30:91–98.
    1. Davis JC, Marra CA, Najafzadeh M, Lui-Ambrose T. The independent contribution of executive functions to health related quality of life in older women. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:16–23.
    1. de Jong R, Coles MGH, Logan GD. Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 1995;21:498–511.
    1. Denson TF, Pederson WC, Friese M, Hahm A, Roberts L. Understanding impulsive aggression: Angry rumination and reduced self-control capacity are mechanisms underlying the provocation-aggression relationship. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011;37:850–62.
    1. D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Cogn. 1999;41:66–86.
    1. Diamond A. Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on A-not-B. Child Dev. 1985;56:868–83.
    1. Diamond A. Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases, of inhibitory control in reaching. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1990;608:637–76.
    1. Diamond A. Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In: Carey S, Gelman R, editors. The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1991. pp. 67–110.
    1. Diamond A. Evidence of robust recognition memory early in life even when assessed by reaching behavior. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1995;59:419–56.
    1. Diamond A. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In: Stuss DT, Knight RT, editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford Univ. Press; London: 2002. pp. 466–503.
    1. Diamond A. Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): a neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity) Dev. Psychopathol. 2005;17:807–25.
    1. Diamond A. All or none hypothesis: a global-default mode that characterizes the brain and mind. Dev. Psychol. 2009;45:130–38.
    1. Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318:1387–88.
    1. Diamond A, Briand L, Fossella J, Gehlbach L. Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive functions in children. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;16:125–32.
    1. Diamond A, Carlson SM, Beck DM. Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the dimensional change card sort task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005;28:689–729.
    1. Diamond A, Kirkham NZ. Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 2005;16:291–97.
    1. Diamond A, Kirkham NZ, Amso D. Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Dev. Psychol. 2002;38:352–62.
    1. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions and programs demonstrated to aid executive function development in children 4–12 years of age. Science. 2011;333:959–64. The only review of EF interventions with children thus far; reviews diverse approaches.

    1. Diamond A, Prevor M, Callender G, Druin DP. Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1997;62(Ser. No. 252):1–7.
    1. Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, et al. School readiness and later achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2007;43:1428–46.
    1. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:475–83.
    1. Duncan J, Parr A, Woolgar A, Thompson R, Bright P, et al. Goal neglect and Spearman’s g: competing parts of a complex task. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2008;137:131–48.
    1. Eakin L, Minde K, Hechtman L, Ochs E, Krane E, et al. The marital and family functioning of adults with ADHD and their spouses. J. Attention Disord. 2004;8:1–10.
    1. Egner T, Hirsch J. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:1784–90.
    1. Eigsti I, Zayas V, Mischel W, Shoda Y, Ayduk O, et al. Predicting cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:478–84.
    1. Eisenberg N, Hofer J, Vaughan C. Effortful control and its socioemotional consequences. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Guilford; New York: 2007. pp. 287–306.
    1. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010;6:495–525.
    1. Eldreth DA, Patterson MD, Porcelli AJ, Biswal BB, Rebbechi D, Rypma B. Evidence for multiple manipulation processes in prefrontal cortex. Brain Res. 2006;1123:145–56.
    1. Engelhardt PE, Nigg JT, Carr LA, Ferreira F. Cognitive inhibition and working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2008;117:591–605.
    1. Engle RW. Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002;11:19–23.
    1. Engle RW, Kane MJ. Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In: Ross B, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Elsevier; New York: 2004. pp. 145–99.
    1. Erickson KL, Kramer AF. Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 2009;43:22–24.
    1. Ericsson KA, Nandagopal K, Roring RW. Toward a science of exceptional achievement: attaining superior performance through deliberate practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009;1172:199–217.
    1. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974;16:143–49.
    1. Espy KA. Using developmental, cognitive, and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2004;26:379–84.
    1. Fairchild G, van Goozen SH, Stollery SJ, Aitken MR, Savage J, et al. Decision making and executive function in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;66:162–68.
    1. Fan J, Flombaum JI, McCandliss BD, Thomas KM, Posner MI. Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage. 2002;18:42–57.
    1. Farran DC, Wilson SJ. Is self-regulation malleable? Results from an evaluation of the Tools of the Mind curriculum. Paper presented at Peabody Res. Inst. Colloquium Ser.; Nashville, TN. 2011.
    1. Feng X, Bialystok E, Diamond A. Bienn. Meet. Soc. Res. Child Dev. Boston, MA: 2007. Manipulating information in working memory: an advantage for bilinguals.
    1. Ferrer E, Shaywitz BA, Holahan JM, Marchione KE, Shaywitz SE. Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychol. Sci. 2009;21:93–101.
    1. Fiore F, Borella E, Mammarella IC, De Beni R. Age differences in verbal and visuo-spatial working memory updating: evidence from analysis of serial position curves. Memory. 2012;20:14–27.
    1. Flook L, Smalley SL, Kitil JM, Galla BM, Kaiser-Greenland S, et al. Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elementary school children. J. Appl. School Psychol. 2010;26:70–95.
    1. Fournet N, Roulin JL, Vallet F, Beaudoin M, Agrigoroaei S, et al. Evaluating short-term and working memory in older adults: French normative data. Aging Ment. Health. 2012;16:922–30.
    1. Frank MJ. Hold your horses: a dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making. Neural Netw. 2006;19:1120–36.
    1. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2004;133:101–35.
    1. Fry AF, Hale S. Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biol. Psychol. 2000;54:1–34.
    1. Gamboz N, Russo R, Fox E. Age differences and the identity negative priming effect: an updated meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging. 2002;17:525–31.
    1. Garavan H, Kelley D, Rosen A, Rao SM, Stein EA. Practice-related functional activation changes in a working memory task. Microsc. Res. Tech. 2000;51:54–63.
    1. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008;134:31–60.
    1. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: evidence from National Curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2004;18:1–16.
    1. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, McEvoy K, Knight RT, D’Esposito M. Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005;17:507–17.
    1. Gazzaley A, Nobre AC. Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:129–35.
    1. Gernsbacher MA, Faust ME. The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. J. Exp. Psychol. 1991;17:245–62.
    1. Georgopoulos AP, Lurito JT, Petrides M, Schwartz AB, Massey JT. Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science. 1989;243:234–36.
    1. Goldsmith HH. Studying temperament via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child Dev. 1996;67:218–35.
    1. Gopnik A, Rosati A. Duck or rabbit? Reversing ambiguous figures and understanding ambiguous representations. Dev. Sci. 2001;4:175–83.
    1. Gupta R, Kar BR, Srinivasan N. Development of task switching and post-error-slowing in children. Behav. Brain Funct. 2009;5:38.
    1. Hale S, Bronik MD, Fry AF. Verbal and spatial working memory in school-age children: developmental differences in susceptibility to interference. Dev. Psychol. 1997;33:364–71.
    1. Hall P, Crossley M, D’Arcy C. Executive function and survival in the context of chronic illness. Ann. Behav. Med. 2010;39:119–27.
    1. Hanania R, Smith LB. Selective attention and attention switching: towards a unified developmental approach. Dev. Sci. 2010;13:622–35.
    1. Hasher L, Stoltzfus ER, Zacks RT, Rypma B. Age and inhibition. J. Exp. Psychol. 1991;17:163–69.
    1. Hasher L, Zacks RT. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view. In: Bower GH, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Academic; San Diego, CA: 1988. pp. 193–225.
    1. Heberle J, Clune M, Kelly K. Bienn. Meet. Soc. Res. Child Dev. Albuquerque, NM: 1999. Development of young children’s understanding of the appearance–reality distinction.
    1. Hedden T, Park D. Aging and interference in verbal working memory. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16:666–81.
    1. Herrigel E. Zen in the Art of Archery. Vintage; New York: 1999.
    1. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65.
    1. Hirt ER, Devers EE, McCrea SM. I want to be creative: exploring the role of hedonic contingency theory in the positive mood–cognitive flexibility link. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008;94:214–30.
    1. Hofmann W, Friese M, Strack F. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2009;4:162–76.
    1. Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:F9–15.
    1. Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Persp. Psychol. Sci. 2011;6:537–59.
    1. Huang YS, Guilleminault C, Li HY, Yang CM, Wu YY, Chen NH. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with obstructive sleep apnea: a treatment outcome study. Sleep Med. 2007;8:18–30.
    1. Ikkai A, Curtis CE. Common neural mechanisms supporting spatial working memory, attention and motor intention. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1428–34.
    1. Jones LB, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. Development of executive attention in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2003;6:498–504.
    1. Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, O’Leary KC, Scudder MR, Wu C-T, et al. The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:1046–58.
    1. Kane MJ, Brown LH, McVay JC, Silvia PJ, Myin-Germeys I, Kwapil TR. For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:614–21.
    1. Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: limits on long-term memory retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. 2000;26:336–58.
    1. Kane MJ, Engle RW. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002;9:637–71.
    1. Karayanidis F, Whitson LR, Heathcote A, Michie PT. Variability in proactive and reactive cognitive control processes across the adult lifespan. Front. Psychol. 2011;2:318.
    1. Karbach J, Kray J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:978–90.
    1. Kendler HH, Kendler TS, Ward JW. An ontogenetic analysis of optional intradimensional and extradimensional shifts. J. Exp. Psychol. 1972;95:102–9.
    1. Kendler TS, Kendler HH. Reversal and nonreversal shifts in kindergarten children. J. Exp. Psychol. 1959;58:56–60.
    1. Kirkham NZ, Cruess L, Diamond A. Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Dev. Sci. 2003;6:449–67.
    1. Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:317–24.
    1. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen P, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–86.
    1. Kloo D, Perner J. Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card sorting task. Dev. Sci. 2005;8:44–56.
    1. Kochanska G, Coy KC, Murray KT. The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Dev. 2001;72:1091–111.
    1. Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA. Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2009;50:1331–38.
    1. Kovács AM, Mehler J. Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:6556–60. This pioneering research demonstrated effects of bilingualism from just listening to two languages without yet speaking.

    1. Kray J. Task-set switching under cue-based versus memory-based switching conditions in younger and older adults. Brain Res. 2006;1105:83–92.
    1. Kray J, Lindenberger U. Adult age differences in task switching. Psychol. Aging. 2000;15:126–47.
    1. Kuo BC, Stokes MG, Nobre AC. Attention modulates maintenance of representations in visual short-term memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;24:51–60.
    1. LaBar KS, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Mesulam M. Neuroanatomic overlap of working memory and spatial attention networks: a functional MRI comparison within subjects. Neuroimage. 1999;10:695–704.
    1. Lakes KD, Hoyt WT. Promoting self-regulation through school-based martial arts training. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2004;25:283–302.
    1. Landau SM, Garavan H, Schumacher EH, D’Esposito M. Regional specificity and practice: dynamic changes in object and spatial working memory. Brain Res. 2007;1180:78–89.
    1. Lehto JE, Juujärvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003;21:59–80.
    1. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 1983.
    1. Liew J. Effortful control, executive functions, and education: bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Dev. Perspect. 2011;6:105–11.
    1. Lillard A, Else-Quest N. The early years: evaluating Montessori education. Science. 2006;313:1893–94.
    1. Liston C, McEwen BS, Casey BJ. Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:912–17.
    1. Louie K, Glimcher PW. Separating value from choice: delay discounting activity in the lateral intraparietal area. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:5498–507.
    1. Lövdén M, Bodammer NC, Kühn S, Kaufmann J, Schütze H, et al. Experience-dependent plasticity of white-matter microstructure extends into old age. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3878–83.
    1. Lu CH, Proctor RW. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1995;2:174–207.
    1. Luciana M, Conklin HM, Hooper CJ, Yarger RS. The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Dev. 2005;76:697–712.
    1. Luciana M, Nelson CA. Assessment of neuropsychological function in children using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB): performance in 4- to 12-year-olds. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2002;22:595–623.
    1. Lui M, Tannock R. Working memory and inattentive behaviour in a community sample of children. Behav. Brain Funct. 2007;3:12.
    1. Luna B. Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2009;37:233–78.
    1. Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. 2004;75:1357–72.
    1. Luna B, Merriam EP, Minshew NJ, Keshavan MS, Genovese CR, et al. Response inhibition improves from late childhood to adulthood: eye movement and fMRI studies. Proc. 6th Annu. Meet. Cogn. Neurosci. Meet.; Washington, DC. 1999.
    1. Lunt L, Bramham J, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Selway RP, et al. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and “jumping to conclusions”: bias or deficit? J. Neuropsychol. 2012;6:65–78.
    1. Mackey AP, Hill SS, Stone SI, Bunge SA. Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in children. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:582–90.
    1. MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 1991;109:163–203.
    1. MacLeod CM, Dodd MD, Sheard ED, Wilson DE, Bibi U. In opposition to inhibition. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 2003;43:163–214.
    1. Manjunath NK, Telles S. Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 2001;45:351–54.
    1. Mason M, Norton M, Van Horn JD, Wegner DW, Grafton ST, Macrae CN. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science. 2007;315:393–95.
    1. Mayr U, Liebscher T. Is there an age deficit in the selection of mental sets? Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2001;13:47–69.
    1. Meiran N. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1996;22:1423–42.
    1. Meiran N, Gotler A. Modeling cognitive control in task switching and aging. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2001;13:165–86.
    1. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2012 In press.
    1. Milham MP, Banich MT, Claus ED, Cohen NJ. Practice-related effects demonstrate complementary roles of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in attentional control. Neuroimage. 2003;18:483–93.
    1. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202.
    1. Miller HV, Barnes JC, Beaver KM. Self-control and health outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Am. J. Health Behav. 2011;35:15–27.
    1. Miller P, Brody CD, Romo R, Wang XJ. A recurrent network model of somatosensory parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1208–18.
    1. Milner B. Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In: Warren JM, Akert K, editors. The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior. McGraw-Hill; New York: 1964. pp. 313–34.
    1. Mischel W, Ayduk O. Self-regulation in a cognitive-affective personality system: attentional control in the service of the self. Self Identity. 2002;1:113–20.
    1. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. Delay of gratification in children. Science. 1989;244:933–38.
    1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000;41:49–100.
    1. Moffitt TE. Childhood self-control predicts adult health, wealth, and crime. Multi-Discipl. Symp. Improv. Well-Being Children Youth; Copenhagen. 2012.
    1. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:2693–98. Best and longest prospective study of EFs, showing they affect health, wealth, and public safety.

    1. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:134–40.
    1. Monsell S, Driver J, editors. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2000. The best single source theory and research task switching (set shifting).

    1. Morrison AB, Chein JM. Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011;18:46–60. Insightful review of EF computerized training that considers methodological problems and what’s needed next.

    1. Morrison FJ, Ponitz CC, McClelland MM. Self-regulation and academic achievement in the transition to school. In: Calkins SD, Bell M, editors. Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition. Am. Psychol. Assoc.; Washington, DC: 2010. pp. 203–24.
    1. Mullane JC, Corkum PV, Klein RM, McLaughlin E. Interference control in children with and without ADHD: a systematic review of Flanker and Simon task performance. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;15:321–42.
    1. Munakata Y, Herd SA, Chatham CH, Depue BE, Banich MT, O’Reilly RC. A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:453–59. Thoughtful articulation of view that there’s no need to postulate a separate inhibitory control function.

    1. Munakata Y, Snyder HR, Chatham CH. Developing cognitive control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012;21:71–77.
    1. Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:218–28.
    1. Munro S, Chau C, Gazarian K, Diamond A. Dramatically larger flanker effects (6-fold elevation). Poster presented at Cogn. Neurosci. Soc. Annu. Meet.; San Francisco, CA. 2006.
    1. Muraven M. Building self-control strength: practicing self-control leads to improved self-control performance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010;46:465–68.
    1. Muraven M, Baumeister RF. Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol. Bull. 2000;126:247–59.
    1. Nelson JM, Sheffield TD, Chevalier N, Clark CAC, Espy KA. Psychobiology of executive function in early development. In: McCardle P, Freund L, Griffin JA, editors. Executive Function in Preschool Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment and Translational Research. Am. Psychol. Assoc.; Washington, DC: 2012. In press.
    1. Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N. Neural mechanisms of attention and control: losing our inhibitions? Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:1631–33.
    1. Nigg JT. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 2000;126:220–46.
    1. Nigg JT, Butler KM, Huang-Pollock CL, Henderson JM. Inhibitory processes in adults with persistent childhood onset ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2002;70:153–57.
    1. Nobre AC, Stokes MG. Attention and short-term memory: crossroads. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1391–92. This special issue explores diverse findings on interrelations between selective attention and short-term memory.

    1. Oaten M, Cheng K. Academic examination stress impairs self-control. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2005;24:254–79.
    1. O’Shaughnessy T, Lane KL, Gresham FM, Beebe-Frankenberger M. Children placed at risk for learning and behavioral difficulties: implementing a school-wide system of early identification and prevention. Remedial Spec. Educ. 2003;24:27–35.
    1. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005;25:46–59.
    1. Owen AM, Morris RG, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Double dissociations of memory and executive functions in a self-ordered working memory task following frontal lobe excision, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Brain. 1996;119:1597–615.
    1. Peltsch A, Hemraj A, Garcia A, Munoz DP. Age-related trends in saccade characteristics among the elderly. Neurobiol. Aging. 2011;32:669–79.
    1. Penadés R, Catalán R, Rubia K, Andrés S, Salamero M, Gastó C. Impaired response inhibition in obsessive compulsive disorder. Eur. Psychiatry. 2007;22:404–10.
    1. Perner J, Lang B. What causes 3-year-olds’ difficulty on the dimensional change card sorting task? Infant Child Dev. 2002;11:93–105.
    1. Petrides M. The effect of periarcuate lesions in the monkey on the performance of symmetrically and asymmetrically reinforced visual and auditory go, no-go tasks. J. Neurosci. 1986;6:2054–63.
    1. Petrides M. Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:359–75.
    1. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Evans AC, Meyer E. Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1993;90:873–77.
    1. Petrides M, Milner B. Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia. 1982;20:249–62.
    1. Piaget J. In: The Child’s Conception of Number. Gattegno C, Hodgson FM, translators. Routledge & Kegan Paul; London: 1952/1941.
    1. Poldrack RA, Sabb FW, Foerde K, Tom SM, Asarnow RF, et al. The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:5356–64.
    1. Posner MI, DiGirolamo GJ. Executive attention: conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. In: Parasuraman R, editor. The Attentive Brain. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 1998. pp. 401–23.
    1. Postle BR, Brush LN, Nick AM. Prefrontal cortex and the mediation of proactive interference in working memory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;4:600–8.
    1. Rachlin H, Ranieri A, Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1991;55:233–44.
    1. Raven J. The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cogn. Psychol. 2000;41:1–48.
    1. Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining CP, Metzger M, Champion KM, Sardin L. Improving preschool classroom processes: preliminary findings from a randomized trial implemented in Head Start settings. Early Child. Res. Q. 2008;23:10–26.
    1. Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, Pressler E. CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Dev. 2011;82:362–78.
    1. Reason J, Mycielska K. Absent-Minded? The Psychology of Mental Lapses and Everyday Errors. Prentice-Hall; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: 1982.
    1. Richmond LL, Morrison AB, Chein JM, Olson IR. Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol. Aging. 2011;26:813–22.
    1. Riggs NR, Greenberg MT, Kusché CA, Pentz MA. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prev. Sci. 2006;7:91–102.
    1. Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D, Sakuma KK, Chou CP, Pentz MA. Executive cognitive function and food intake in children. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2010;42:398–403.
    1. Riviere J, Lecuyer R. The C-not-B error: a comparative study. Cogn. Dev. 2003;18:285–97.
    1. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Lawrence AD, et al. A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998;4:474–90.
    1. Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1988;40B:321–41.
    1. Roberts RJ, Pennington BF. An interactive framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1996;12:105–26.
    1. Roca M, Parr A, Thompson R, Woolgar A, Torralva T, et al. Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain. 2010;133:234–47.
    1. Rogers RD, Monsell S. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 1995;124:207–31.
    1. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at 3–7 years: the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001;72:1394–408.
    1. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 3: Social Emotional and Personality Development. Wiley; New York: 2006. pp. 105–76.
    1. Rozas AX, Juncos-Rabadán O, González MS. Processing speed, inhibitory control, and working memory: three important factors to account for age-related cognitive decline. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2008;66:115–30.
    1. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005;28:573–94.
    1. Rutman AM, Clapp WC, Chadick JZ, Gazzaley A. Early top-down control of visual processing predicts working memory performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:1224–34.
    1. Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald A, Levy R, et al. A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1988;111:695–718.
    1. Salthouse TA. Influence of processing speed on adult age differences in working memory. Acta Psychol. 1992;79:155–70.
    1. Sethi A, Mischel W, Aber J, Shoda Y, Rodriguez M. The role of strategic attention deployment in development of self-regulation: predicting preschoolers’ delay of gratification from mother-toddler interactions. Dev. Psychol. 2000;36:767–77.
    1. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Is working memory training effective? Psychol. Bull. 2012;138:628–54. Insightful review of computerized EF training that raises important methodological considerations.

    1. Simpson A, Riggs KJ. Under what conditions do young children have difficulty inhibiting manual actions? Dev. Psychol. 2007;43:417–28.
    1. Simpson A, Riggs KJ, Beck SR, Gorniak SL, Wu Y, et al. Refining the understanding of inhibitory control: how response prepotency is created and overcome. Dev. Sci. 2012;15:62–73.
    1. Smallwood J, Schooler JW. In: Mind-wandering. In The Oxford Companion to Consciousness. Bayne T, Cleermans A, Wilken P, editors. Oxford Univ. Press; Oxford, UK: 2009. pp. 443–45.
    1. Smith EE, Jonides J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science. 1999;283:1657–61.
    1. Solesio-Jofre E, Lorenzo-López L, Gutiérrez R, Lópezv-Frutos JM, Ruiz-Vargas JM, Maestú F. Age-related effects in working memory recognition modulated by retroactive interference. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. 2012;67:565–72.
    1. Stedron JM, Sahni SD, Munakata Y. Common mechanisms for working memory and attention: the case of perseveration with visible solutions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005;17:623–31.
    1. Stuss DT, Levine B, Alexander MP, Hong J, Palumbo C, et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:388–402.
    1. Sweeney JA, Rosano C, Berman RA, Luna B. Inhibitory control of attention declines more than working memory during normal aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 2001;22:39–47.
    1. Taylor Tavares JV, Clark L, Cannon DM, Erickson K, Drevets WC, Sahakian BJ. Distinct profiles of neurocognitive function in unmedicated unipolar depression and bipolar II depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:917–24.
    1. Theeuwes J. Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: the effect of visual onsets and offsets. Percept. Psychophys. 1991;49:83–90.
    1. Theeuwes J. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychol. 2010;315:77–99.
    1. Thorell LB, Lindqvist S, Bergman N, Bohlin G, Klingberg T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:106–13.
    1. Tun PA, Miller-Martinez D, Lachman ME, Seeman T. Social strain and executive function across the lifespan: the dark (and light) sides of social engagement. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. 2012 In press.
    1. Valle-Inclán F. The locus of interference in the Simon effect: an ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 1996;43:147–62.
    1. Van der Elst W, Hurks P, Wassenberg R, Meijs C, Jolles J. Animal verbal fluency and design fluency in school-aged children: effects of age, sex, and mean level of parental education, and regression-based normative data. J. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2011;33:1005–15.
    1. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 2008;137:649–72.
    1. Verhaeghen P, Basak C. Ageing and switching of the focus of attention in working memory: results from a modified N-back task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2005;58:134–54.
    1. von Hecker U, Meiser T. Defocused attention in depressed mood: evidence from source monitoring. Emotion. 2005;5:456–63.
    1. Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the lifespan. J. Appl. Physiol. 2011;111:1505–13.
    1. Wais P, Gazzaley A. The impact of auditory distraction on retrieval of visual memories. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011;18:1090–97.
    1. Wais P, Rubens M, Boccanfuso J, Gazzaley A. Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of visual distraction on retrieval of long-term memory. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:8541–50.
    1. Wass S, Porayska-Pomsta K, Johnson MH. Training attentional control in infancy. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:1–5.
    1. Wendelken C, Munakata Y, Baym C, Souza M, Bunge S. Flexible rule use: common neural substrates in children and adults. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;2:329–39.
    1. Wiebe SA, Lukowski AF, Bauer PJ. Sequence imitation and reaching measures of executive control: a longitudinal examination in the second year of life. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010;35:522–38.
    1. Wright A, Diamond A. Dissociating working memory and inhibition: an effect of inhibitory load while keeping working memory load constant. 2012 Manuscript submitted.
    1. Zacks RT, Hasher L. Aging and long-term memory: Deficits are not inevitable. In: Bialystock E, Craik FIM, editors. Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change. Oxford Univ. Press; New York: 2006. pp. 162–77.
    1. Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies optimal working memory performance. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:3059–66.
    1. Zanto TP, Hennigan K, Östberg M, Clapp WC, Gazzaley A. Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults. Cortex. 2010;46:564–74.
    1. Zanto TP, Rubens MT, Thangavel A, Gazzaley A. Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:656–61.
    1. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Diamond BJ, David Z, Goolkasian P. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Conscious. Cogn. 2010;19:597–605.
    1. Zelazo PD, Frye D, Rapus T. An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cogn. Dev. 1996;11:37–63.
    1. Zelazo PD, Mueller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2003;68:1–137.
    1. Zimprich D, Kurtz T. Individual differences and predictors of forgetting in old age: the role of processing speed and working memory. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. 2012 In press.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren