Sweet taste liking is associated with impulsive behaviors in humans

Jessica Weafer, Anne Burkhardt, Harriet de Wit, Jessica Weafer, Anne Burkhardt, Harriet de Wit

Abstract

Evidence from both human and animal studies suggests that sensitivity to rewarding stimuli is positively associated with impulsive behaviors, including both impulsive decision making and inhibitory control. The current study examined associations between the hedonic value of a sweet taste and two forms of impulsivity (impulsive choice and impulsive action) in healthy young adults (N = 100). Participants completed a sweet taste test in which they rated their liking of various sweetness concentrations. Subjects also completed measures of impulsive choice (delay discounting), and impulsive action (go/no-go task). Subjects who discounted more steeply (i.e., greater impulsive choice) liked the high sweetness concentration solutions more. By contrast, sweet liking was not related to impulsive action. These findings indicate that impulsive choice may be associated with heightened sensitivity to the hedonic value of a rewarding stimulus, and that these constructs might share common underlying neurobiological mechanisms.

Keywords: decision-making; delay discounting; impulsive action; impulsive choice; reward sensitivity; sucrose; sweet taste.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean liking ratings of each of the five sweetness concentrations for sweet likers and sweet dislikers. ** indicates p < 0.01 and *** indicates p < 0.001. Capped vertical lines represent standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean indifference points at each delay for sweet likers and sweet dislikers. * indicates p < 0.05. Capped vertical lines represent standard error of the mean (SEM).

References

    1. Acheson A., Vincent A. S., Sorocco K. H., Lovallo W. R. (2011). Greater discounting of delayed rewards in young adults with family histories of alcohol and drug use disorders: studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 1607–1613 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01507.x
    1. American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association
    1. Anker J. J., Gliddon L. A., Carroll M. E. (2008). Impulsivity on a Go/No-go task for intravenous cocaine or food in male and female rats selectively bred for high and low saccharin intake. Behav. Pharmacol. 19, 615–629 10.1097/fbp.0b013e32830dc0ae
    1. Anker J. J., Perry J. L., Gliddon L. A., Carroll M. E. (2009). Impulsivity predicts the escalation of cocaine self-administration in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 343–348 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.05.013
    1. Babor T. F., Kranzler H. R., Lauerman R. J. (1989). Early detection of harmful alcohol consumption: comparison of clinical, laboratory and self-report screening procedures. Addict. Behav. 14, 139–157 10.1016/0306-4603(89)90043-9
    1. Bartra O., Mcguire J. T., Kable J. W. (2013). The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage 76, 412–427 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063
    1. Beck R. C., Triplett M. F. (2009). Test-retest reliability of a group-administered paper-pencil measure of delay discounting. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 17, 345–355 10.1037/a0017078
    1. Bickel W. K., Koffarnus M. N., Moody L., Wilson A. G. (2014). The behavioral- and neuro-economic process of temporal discounting: a candidate behavioral marker of addiction. Neuropharmacology 76(Pt. B), 518–527 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.06.013
    1. Bickel W. K., Yi R., Landes R. D., Hill P. F., Baxter C. (2011). Remembering the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 260–265 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017
    1. Bjork J. M., Hommer D. W., Grant S. J., Danube C. (2004). Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol-dependent patients: relation to control subjects and type 1-/type 2-like traits. Alcohol 34, 133–150 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.06.012
    1. Carroll M. E., Holtz N. A., Zlebnik N. E. (2013). “Saccharin preference in rats: relation to impulsivity and drug abuse,” in Animal Models of Eating Disorders, Neuromethods, ed Avena N. M. (Totowa, Heidelberg, New York: Springer/Humana; ), 201–233
    1. Cervantes M. C., Laughlin R. E., Jentsch J. D. (2013). Cocaine self-administration behavior in inbred mouse lines segregating different capacities for inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 229, 515–525 10.1007/s00213-013-3135-4
    1. Dalley J. W., Fryer T. D., Brichard L., Robinson E. S., Theobald D. E., Laane K., et al. (2007). Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315, 1267–1270 10.1126/science.1137073
    1. Diergaarde L., Pattij T., Nawijn L., Schoffelmeer A. N., De Vries T. J. (2009). Trait impulsivity predicts escalation of sucrose seeking and hypersensitivity to sucrose-associated stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 794–803 10.1037/a0016504
    1. Diergaarde L., Pattij T., Poortvliet I., Hogenboom F., De Vries W., Schoffelmeer A. N., et al. (2008). Impulsive choice and impulsive action predict vulnerability to distinct stages of nicotine seeking in rats. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 301–308 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.011
    1. Fillmore M. T., Rush C. R. (2002). Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 66, 265–273 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00206-x
    1. Herting M. M., Schwartz D., Mitchell S. H., Nagel B. J. (2010). Delay discounting behavior and white matter microstructure abnormalities in youth with a family history of alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 1590–1602 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01244.x
    1. Jentsch J. D., Ashenhurst J. R., Cervantes M. C., Groman S. M., James A. S., Pennington Z. T. (2014). Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1111/nyas.12388
    1. Jentsch J. D., Pennington Z. T. (2014). Reward, interrupted: inhibitory control and its relevance to addictions. Neuropharmacology 76(Pt. B), 479–486 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.022
    1. Kampov-Polevoy A., Garbutt J. C., Janowsky D. (1997). Evidence of preference for a high-concentration sucrose solution in alcoholic men. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 269–270
    1. Kampov-Polevoy A. B., Garbutt J. C., Khalitov E. (2003). Family history of alcoholism and response to sweets. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 27, 1743–1749 10.1097/01.alc.0000093739.05809.dd
    1. Kampov-Polevoy A. B., Tsoi M. V., Zvartau E. E., Neznanov N. G., Khalitov E. (2001). Sweet liking and family history of alcoholism in hospitalized alcoholic and non-alcoholic patients. Alcohol Alcohol. 36, 165–170 10.1093/alcalc/36.2.165
    1. Kareken D. A., Dzemidzic M., Oberlin B. G., Eiler W. J., 2nd (2013). A preliminary study of the human brain response to oral sucrose and its association with recent drinking. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 2058–2065 10.1111/acer.12194
    1. Landes R. D., Christensen D. R., Bickel W. R. (2012). Delay discounting decreases in those completing treatment for opioid dependence. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 20, 302–309 10.1037/a0027391
    1. MacKillop J., Amlung M. T., Few L. R., Ray L. A., Sweet L. H., Munafo M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 216, 305–321 10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0
    1. Mitchell S. H. (2011). The genetic basis of delay discounting and its genetic relationship to alcohol dependence. Behav. Processes 87, 10–17 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.02.008
    1. Monterosso J. R., Aron A. R., Cordova X., Xu J., London E. D. (2005). Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 79, 273–277 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.02.002
    1. Oberlin B. G., Grahame N. J. (2009). High-alcohol preferring mice are more impulsive than low-alcohol preferring mice as measured in the delay discounting task. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 1294–1303 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00955.x
    1. Ohmura Y., Takahashi T., Kitamura N., Wehr P. (2006). Three-month stability of delay and probability discounting measures. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 14, 318–328 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.318
    1. Perry J. L., Larson E. B., German J. P., Madden G. J., Carroll M. E. (2005). Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 178, 193–201 10.1007/s00213-004-1994-4
    1. Perry J. L., Nelson S. E., Anderson M. M., Morgan A. D., Carroll M. E. (2007). Impulsivity (delay discounting) for food and cocaine in male and female rats selectively bred for high and low saccharin intake. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 86, 822–837 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.012
    1. Peters J., Büchel C. (2011). The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 227–239 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.002
    1. Petry N. M., Kirby K. N., Kranzler H. R. (2002). Effects of gender and family history of alcohol dependence on a behavioral task of impulsivity in healthy subjects. J. Stud. Alcohol 63, 83–90
    1. Poulos C. X., Le A. D., Parker J. L. (1995). Impulsivity predicts individual susceptibility to high levels of alcohol self-administration. Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 810–814 10.1097/00008877-199512000-00006
    1. Rubio G., Jimenez M., Rodriguez-Jimenez R., Martinez I., Avila C., Ferre F., et al. (2008). The role of behavioral impulsivity in the development of alcohol dependence: a 4-year follow-up study. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 1681–1687 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00746.x
    1. Rudenga K. J., Small D. M. (2013). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex response to concentrated sucrose reflects liking rather than sweet quality coding. Chem. Senses 38, 585–594 10.1093/chemse/bjt029
    1. Sellitto M., Ciaramelli E., Di Pellegrino G. (2010). Myopic discounting of future rewards after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. J. Neurosci. 30, 16429–16436 10.1523/jneurosci.2516-10.2010
    1. Sinclair J. D., Kampov-Polevoy A., Stewart R., Li T. K. (1992). Taste preferences in rat lines selected for low and high alcohol consumption. Alcohol 9, 155–160 10.1016/0741-8329(92)90027-8
    1. Sobell L. C., Sobell M. B. (1992). “Timeline follow-back: a technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption,” in Measuring Alcohol Consumption: Psychosocial and Biochemical Methods, eds Litten R. Z., Allen J. P. (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; ), 41–72
    1. Stephens D. N., Duka T., Crombag H. S., Cunningham C. L., Heilig M., Crabbe J. C. (2010). Reward sensitivity: issues of measurement and achieving consilience between human and animal phenotypes. Addict. Biol. 15, 145–168 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00193.x
    1. Stewart R. B., Russell R. N., Lumeng L., Li T. K., Murphy J. M. (1994). Consumption of sweet, salty, sour and bitter solutions by selectively bred alcohol-preferring and alcohol-nonpreferring lines of rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 18, 375–381 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1994.tb00028.x
    1. Weafer J., De Wit H. (2013). Inattention, impulsive action and subjective response to D-amphetamine. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133, 127–133 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.05.021
    1. Wilhelm C. J., Mitchell S. H. (2008). Rats bred for high alcohol drinking are more sensitive to delayed and probabilistic outcomes. Genes Brain Behav. 7, 705–713 10.1111/j.1601-183x.2008.00406.x
    1. Winstanley C. A. (2011). The utility of rat models of impulsivity in developing pharmacotherapies for impulse control disorders. Br. J. Pharmacol. 164, 1301–1321 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01323.x

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren