Positive association of video game playing with left frontal cortical thickness in adolescents

Simone Kühn, Robert Lorenz, Tobias Banaschewski, Gareth J Barker, Christian Büchel, Patricia J Conrod, Herta Flor, Hugh Garavan, Bernd Ittermann, Eva Loth, Karl Mann, Frauke Nees, Eric Artiges, Tomas Paus, Marcella Rietschel, Michael N Smolka, Andreas Ströhle, Bernadetta Walaszek, Gunter Schumann, Andreas Heinz, Jürgen Gallinat, IMAGEN Consortium, Simone Kühn, Robert Lorenz, Tobias Banaschewski, Gareth J Barker, Christian Büchel, Patricia J Conrod, Herta Flor, Hugh Garavan, Bernd Ittermann, Eva Loth, Karl Mann, Frauke Nees, Eric Artiges, Tomas Paus, Marcella Rietschel, Michael N Smolka, Andreas Ströhle, Bernadetta Walaszek, Gunter Schumann, Andreas Heinz, Jürgen Gallinat, IMAGEN Consortium

Abstract

Playing video games is a common recreational activity of adolescents. Recent research associated frequent video game playing with improvements in cognitive functions. Improvements in cognition have been related to grey matter changes in prefrontal cortex. However, a fine-grained analysis of human brain structure in relation to video gaming is lacking. In magnetic resonance imaging scans of 152 14-year old adolescents, FreeSurfer was used to estimate cortical thickness. Cortical thickness across the whole cortical surface was correlated with self-reported duration of video gaming (hours per week). A robust positive association between cortical thickness and video gaming duration was observed in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and left frontal eye fields (FEFs). No regions showed cortical thinning in association with video gaming frequency. DLPFC is the core correlate of executive control and strategic planning which in turn are essential cognitive domains for successful video gaming. The FEFs are a key region involved in visuo-motor integration important for programming and execution of eye movements and allocation of visuo-spatial attention, processes engaged extensively in video games. The results may represent the biological basis of previously reported cognitive improvements due to video game play. Whether or not these results represent a-priori characteristics or consequences of video gaming should be studied in future longitudinal investigations.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Significant clusters of the cortical…
Figure 1. Significant clusters of the cortical thickness correlation with hours of video gaming per week in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and left frontal eye fields (FEF) (multiple comparison corrected, p<0.01).
Figure 2. Scatter plot of the association…
Figure 2. Scatter plot of the association between cortical thickness in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, top) and frontal eye fields (FEF, bottom) and hours of video gaming per week.
Correlation coefficients are not reported since the brain regions are defined based on a whole brain analysis and applying statistics on these regions could be considered “double dipping” (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009).

References

    1. Anderson CA, Shibuya A, Ihori N, Swing EL, Bushman BJ, et al. (2010) Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 136: 151–173.
    1. Kirsh SJ, Mounts JR (2007) Violent video game play impacts facial emotion recognition. Aggress Behav 33: 353–358.
    1. Green CS, Pouget A, Bavelier D (2010) Improved probabilistic inference as a general learning mechanism with action video games. Curr Biol 20: 1573–1579.
    1. Green CS, Bavelier D (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423: 534–537.
    1. Green CS, Bavelier D (2006) Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players. Cognition 101: 217–245.
    1. Green CS, Bavelier D (2007) Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychol Sci 18: 88–94.
    1. Li R, Polat U, Makous W, Bavelier D (2009) Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training. Nat Neurosci 12: 549–551.
    1. Basak C, Boot WR, Voss MW, Kramer AF (2008) Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol Aging 23: 765–777.
    1. Spence I, Yu JJ, Feng J, Marshman J (2009) Women match men when learning a spatial skill. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35: 1097–1103.
    1. Lynch J, Aughwane P, Hammond TM (2010) Video games and surgical ability: a literature review. J Surg Educ 67: 184–189.
    1. Schlickum MK, Hedman L, Enochsson L, Kjellin A, Fellander-Tsai L (2009) Systematic video game training in surgical novices improves performance in virtual reality endoscopic surgical simulators: a prospective randomized study. World J Surg 33: 2360–2367.
    1. Haslinger B, Erhard P, Altenmuller E, Hennenlotter A, Schwaiger M, et al. (2004) Reduced recruitment of motor association areas during bimanual coordination in concert pianists. Hum Brain Mapp 22: 206–215.
    1. Smith ME, McEvoy LK, Gevins A (1999) Neurophysiological indices of strategy development and skill acquisition. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 7: 389–404.
    1. Granert O, Peller M, Gaser C, Groppa S, Hallett M, et al. (2011) Manual activity shapes structure and function in contralateral human motor hand area. Neuroimage 54: 32–41.
    1. Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, et al. (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 4398–4403.
    1. Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, et al. (2004) Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427: 311–312.
    1. Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Winkler J, et al. (2006) Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci 26: 6314–6317.
    1. Cannonieri GC, Bonilha L, Fernandes PT, Cendes F, Li LM (2007) Practice and perfect: length of training and structural brain changes in experienced typists. Neuroreport 18: 1063–1066.
    1. Schmidt-Wilcke T, Rosengarth K, Luerding R, Bogdahn U, Greenlee MW (2010) Distinct patterns of functional and structural neuroplasticity associated with learning Morse code. Neuroimage 51: 1234–1241.
    1. Groussard M, La Joie R, Rauchs G, Landeau B, Chetelat G, et al.. (2010) When music and long-term memory interact: effects of musical expertise on functional and structural plasticity in the hippocampus. PLoS One 5..
    1. Lövden M, Schaefer S, Noack H, Bodammer NC, Kuhn S, et al.. (2012) Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood. Neurobiol Aging 33 : 620 e629–620 e622.
    1. Kuhn S, Romanowski A, Schilling C, Lorenz R, Morsen C, et al. (2011) The neural basis of video gaming. Transl Psychiatry 1: e53.
    1. Choi YY, Shamosh NA, Cho SH, DeYoung CG, Lee MJ, et al. (2008) Multiple bases of human intelligence revealed by cortical thickness and neural activation. J Neurosci 28: 10323–10329.
    1. Dickerson BC, Fenstermacher E, Salat DH, Wolk DA, Maguire RP, et al. (2008) Detection of cortical thickness correlates of cognitive performance: Reliability across MRI scan sessions, scanners, and field strengths. Neuroimage 39: 10–18.
    1. Hutton C, Draganski B, Ashburner J, Weiskopf N (2009) A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging. Neuroimage 48: 371–380.
    1. Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, et al. (2004) Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb Cortex 14: 721–730.
    1. Schumann G, Loth E, Banaschewski T, Barbot A, Barker G, et al. (2010) The IMAGEN study: reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 15: 1128–1139.
    1. Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H (2000) The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41: 645–655.
    1. Wolfling K, Muller KW, Beutel M (2011) [Reliability and validity of the Scale for the Assessment of Pathological Computer-Gaming (CSV-S)]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 61: 216–224.
    1. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9: 179–194.
    1. Fischl B, Dale AM (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 11050–11055.
    1. Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-Planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: NY: Thieme Medical Publishers Inc.
    1. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, et al. (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31: 968–980.
    1. Grosbras MH, Laird AR, Paus T (2005) Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 140–154.
    1. Paus T (1996) Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review. Neuropsychologia 34: 475–483.
    1. Miller EK (2000) The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 59–65.
    1. Petrides M (2005) Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 781–795.
    1. Krawczyk DC (2002) Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 631–664.
    1. Fuster JM (2000) Executive frontal functions. Exp Brain Res 133: 66–70.
    1. Levy R, Goldman-Rakic PS (2000) Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res 133: 23–32.
    1. Petrides M, Milner B (1982) Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 20: 249–262.
    1. Goldberg E, Harner R, Lovell M, Podell K, Riggio S (1994) Cognitive bias, functional cortical geometry, and the frontal lobes: laterality, sex, and handedness. J Cogn Neurosci 6: 276–296.
    1. Goel V, Dolan RJ (2000) Anatomical segregation of component processes in an inductive inference task. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 110–119.
    1. Kim JN, Shadlen MN (1999) Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Nat Neurosci 2: 176–185.
    1. Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Bandettini PA, Ungerleider LG (2004) A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature 431: 859–862.
    1. Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Ruff DA, Bandettini PA, Ungerleider LG (2006) Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 10023–10028.
    1. Petrides M (1985) Deficits in non-spatial conditional associative learning after periarcuate lesions in the monkey. Behav Brain Res 16: 95–101.
    1. Lobel E, Kahane P, Leonards U, Grosbras M, Lehericy S, et al. (2001) Localization of human frontal eye fields: anatomical and functional findings of functional magnetic resonance imaging and intracerebral electrical stimulation. J Neurosurg 95: 804–815.
    1. Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Ungerleider LG (2008) The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 467–479.
    1. Castel AD, Pratt J, Drummond E (2005) The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. Acta Psychol (Amst) 119: 217–230.
    1. Li RW, Levi DM, Klein SA (2004) Perceptual learning improves efficiency by re-tuning the decision ‘template’ for position discrimination. Nat Neurosci 7: 178–183.
    1. Petrov AA, Dosher BA, Lu ZL (2005) The dynamics of perceptual learning: an incremental reweighting model. Psychol Rev 112: 715–743.
    1. Gopher D, Weil M, Bareket T (1994) Transfer of skill from a computer game trainer to flight. Human Factors 36: 387–405.
    1. Fahle M (2005) Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 154–160.
    1. Lustig C, Shah P, Seidler R, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2009) Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol Rev 19: 504–522.
    1. Kühn S, Gleich T, Lorenz RC, Lindenberger U, Gallinat J (2013) Playing Super Mario induces structural brain plasticity: grey matter changes resulting from training with a commercial video game. Mol Psychiatry.
    1. Yuan K, Cheng P, Dong T, Bi Y, Xing L, et al. (2013) Cortical thickness abnormalities in late adolescence with online gaming addiction. PLoS One 8: e53055.

Source: PubMed

3
Suscribir