Differential frontal alpha oscillations and mechanisms underlying loss of consciousness: a comparison between slow and fast propofol infusion rates

P Sepúlveda, L I Cortinez, M Irani, J I Egaña, V Contreras, A Sánchez Corzo, I Acosta, R Sitaram, P Sepúlveda, L I Cortinez, M Irani, J I Egaña, V Contreras, A Sánchez Corzo, I Acosta, R Sitaram

Abstract

Mechanisms underlying loss of consciousness following propofol administration remain incompletely understood. The objective of this study was to compare frontal lobe electroencephalography activity and brainstem reflexes during intravenous induction of general anaesthesia, in patients receiving a typical bolus dose (fast infusion) of propofol compared with a slower infusion rate. We sought to determine whether brainstem suppression ('bottom-up') predominates over loss of cortical function ('top-down'). Sixteen ASA physical status-1 patients were randomly assigned to either a fast or slow propofol infusion group. Loss of consciousness and brainstem reflexes were assessed every 30 s by a neurologist blinded to treatment allocation. We performed a multitaper spectral analysis of all electroencephalography data obtained from each participant. Brainstem reflexes were present in all eight patients in the slow infusion group, while being absent in all patients in the fast infusion group, at the moment of loss of consciousness (p = 0.010). An increase in alpha band power was observed before loss of consciousness only in participants allocated to the slow infusion group. Alpha band power emerged several minutes after the loss of consciousness in participants allocated to the fast infusion group. Our results show a predominance of 'bottom-up' mechanisms during fast infusion rates and 'top-down' mechanisms during slow infusion rates. The underlying mechanisms by which propofol induces loss of consciousness are potentially influenced by the speed of infusion.

Keywords: EEG; alpha band; loss of consciousness; propofol; speed of induction.

© 2019 Association of Anaesthetists.

References

    1. Kaskinoro K, Maksimow A, Langsjo J, et al. Wide inter-individual variability of bispectral index and spectral entropy at loss of consciousness during increasing concentrations of dexmedetomidine, propofol, and sevoflurane. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2011; 107: 573-80.
    1. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 2008; 322: 876-80.
    1. Struys MM, Coppens MJ, De Neve N, et al. Influence of administration rate on propofol plasma-effect site equilibration. Anesthesiology 2007; 107: 386-96.
    1. Tinker JH, Sharbrough FW, Michenfelder JD. Anterior shift of the dominant EEG rhytham during anesthesia in the Java monkey: correlation with anesthetic potency. Anesthesiology 1977; 46: 252-9.
    1. Sloan TB. Anesthetic effects on electrophysiologic recordings. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 1998; 15: 217-26.
    1. Purdon PL, Pavone KJ, Akeju O, et al. The Ageing Brain: age-dependent changes in the electroencephalogram during propofol and sevoflurane general anaesthesia. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2015; 115(Suppl. 1): i46-57.
    1. Gajraj RJ, Doi M, Mantzaridis H, Kenny GN. Analysis of the EEG bispectrum, auditory evoked potentials and the EEG power spectrum during repeated transitions from consciousness to unconsciousness. British Journal of Anaesthesia 1998; 80: 46-52.
    1. Constant I, Sabourdin N. The EEG signal: a window on the cortical brain activity. Paediatric Anesthesia 2012; 22: 539-52.
    1. Cimenser A, Purdon PL, Pierce ET, et al. Tracking brain states under general anesthesia by using global coherence analysis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2011; 108: 8832-7.
    1. Mukamel EA, Pirondini E, Babadi B, et al. A Transition in Brain State during Propofol-Induced Unconsciousness. Journal of Neuroscience 2014; 34: 839.
    1. Lee U, Ku S, Noh G, Baek S, Choi B, Mashour GA. Disruption of frontal-parietal communication by ketamine, propofol, and sevoflurane. Anesthesiology 2013; 118: 1264-75.
    1. Flores FJ, Hartnack KE, Fath AB, et al. Thalamocortical synchronization during induction and emergence from propofol-induced unconsciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2017; 114: E6660-E8.
    1. Hagihira S. Brain mechanisms during course of anesthesia: what we know from EEG changes during induction and recovery. Frontiers in System Neuroscience 2017; 11: 39.
    1. Mashour GA, Hudetz AG. Bottom-up and top-down mechanisms of general anesthetics modulate different dimensions of consciousness. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 2017; 11: 44.
    1. Smith C, McEwan AI, Jhaveri R, et al. The interaction of fentanyl on the Cp50 of propofol for loss of consciousness and skin incision. Anesthesiology 1994; 81: 820-8.
    1. Iyer VN, Mandrekar JN, Danielson RD, Zubkov AY, Elmer JL, Wijdicks EF. Validity of the FOUR score coma scale in the medical intensive care unit. Mayo Clinic Proceedings 2009; 84: 694-701.
    1. Purdon PL, Pierce ET, Mukamel EA, et al. Electroencephalogram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness from propofol. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2013; 110: E1142-51.
    1. Gugino LD, Chabot RJ, Prichep LS, John ER, Formanek V, Aglio LS. Quantitative EEG changes associated with loss and return of consciousness in healthy adult volunteers anaesthetized with propofol or sevoflurane. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2001; 87: 421-8.
    1. Ching S, Cimenser A, Purdon PL, Brown EN, Kopell NJ. Thalamocortical model for a propofol-induced alpha-rhythm associated with loss of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2010; 107: 22665-70.
    1. Flores FJ, Ching S, Hartnack K, et al. A PK-PD model of ketamine-induced high-frequency oscillations. Journal of Neural Engineering 2015; 12: 056006.
    1. Rudolph U, Antkowiak B. Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2004; 5: 709-20.
    1. Brown EN, Purdon PL, Van Dort CJ. General anesthesia and altered states of arousal: a systems neuroscience analysis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 2011; 34: 601-28.
    1. Franks NP. General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2008; 9: 370-86.
    1. Brown EN, Lydic R, Schiff ND. General anesthesia, sleep, and coma. New England Journal of Medicine 2010; 363: 2638-50.
    1. Liu X, Lauer KK, Ward BD, Li SJ, Hudetz AG. Differential effects of deep sedation with propofol on the specific and nonspecific thalamocortical systems: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Anesthesiology 2013; 118: 59-69.
    1. Mashour GA. Cognitive unbinding: a neuroscientific paradigm of general anesthesia and related states of unconsciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2013; 37: 2751-9.
    1. Bilotta F, Doronzio A, Stazi E, et al. Early postoperative cognitive dysfunction and postoperative delirium after anaesthesia with various hypnotics: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial-the PINOCCHIO trial. Trials 2011; 12: 170.
    1. de Wit F, van Vliet AL, de Wilde RB, et al. The effect of propofol on haemodynamics: cardiac output, venous return, mean systemic filling pressure, and vascular resistances. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2016; 116: 784-9.
    1. Smith WD, Dutton RC, Smith NT. Measuring the performance of anesthetic depth indicators. Anesthesiology 1996; 84: 38-51.
    1. Pilge S, Zanner R, Schneider G, Blum J, Kreuzer M, Kochs EF. Time delay of index calculation: analysis of cerebral state, bispectral, and narcotrend indices. Anesthesiology 2006; 104: 488-94.
    1. Coppens M, Van Limmen JG, Schnider T, et al. Study of the time course of the clinical effect of propofol compared with the time course of the predicted effect-site concentration: performance of three pharmacokinetic-dynamic models. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2010; 104: 452-8.

Source: PubMed

3
Suscribir