A randomized controlled trial testing the efficacy of a Nurse Home Visiting Program for Pregnant Adolescents

Daniel Fatori, Pedro Fonseca Zuccolo, Elizabeth Shephard, Helena Brentani, Alicia Matijasevich, Alexandre Archanjo Ferraro, Lislaine Aparecida Fracolli, Anna Maria Chiesa, James Leckman, Euripedes Constantino Miguel, Guilherme V Polanczyk, Daniel Fatori, Pedro Fonseca Zuccolo, Elizabeth Shephard, Helena Brentani, Alicia Matijasevich, Alexandre Archanjo Ferraro, Lislaine Aparecida Fracolli, Anna Maria Chiesa, James Leckman, Euripedes Constantino Miguel, Guilherme V Polanczyk

Abstract

To test the efficacy of a nurse home visiting program (HVP) on child development, maternal and environmental outcomes in the first years of life. We conducted a randomized controlled trial to test the efficacy of Primeiros Laços, a nurse HVP for adolescent mothers living in a poor urban area of São Paulo, Brazil. Eighty adolescent mothers were included and randomized to receive either Primeiros Laços (intervention group, n = 40) or healthcare as usual (control group, n = 40). Primeiros Laços is a home visiting intervention delivered by trained nurses that starts during the first 16 weeks of pregnancy and continues to the child's age of 24 months. Participants were assessed by blind interviewers at 8-16 weeks of pregnancy (baseline), 30 weeks of pregnancy, and 3, 6, 12, and 24 months of child's age. We assessed oscillatory power in the mid-range alpha frequency via electroencephalography when the children were aged 6 months. Child development was measured by the Bayley Scales of Infant Development Third Edition (BSID-III). Weight and length were measured by trained professionals and anthropometric indexes were calculated. The home environment and maternal interaction with the child was measured by the Home Observation and Measurement of the Environment. Generalized estimating equation models were used to examine intervention effects on the trajectories of outcomes. Standardized effect sizes (Cohen's d) were calculated using marginal means from endpoint assessments of all outcomes. The trial was registered at clinicaltrial.gov: NCT02807818. Our analyses showed significant positive effects of the intervention on child expressive language development (coefficient = 0.89, 95% CI [0.18, 1.61], p = 0.014), maternal emotional/verbal responsivity (coefficient = 0.97, 95% CI [0.37, 1.58], p = 0.002), and opportunities for variety in daily stimulation (coefficient = 0.37, 95% CI [0.09, 0.66], p = 0.009). Standardized effect sizes of the intervention were small to moderate. Primeiros Laços is a promising intervention to promote child development and to improve the home environment of low-income adolescent mothers. However, considering the limitations of our study, future studies should be conducted to assess Primeiros Laços potential to benefit this population.Clinical Trial Registration: The study was registered at clinicaltrial.gov (Registration date: 21/06/2016 and Registration number: NCT02807818).

Conflict of interest statement

Guilherme Polanczyk has been in the past 3 years a member of advisory board of Shire/Takeda and Medice and a speaker for Shire/Takeda, Novo Nordisk and Aché. He received travel expenses for continuing education support from Shire/Takeda and royalties from Editora Manole. The other authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

© 2021. The Author(s).

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
CONSORT flow diagram of the randomized controlled trial.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Intervention effects on child alpha power at age 6 months measured by EEG (intervention n = 17, control n = 14). Fitted model plot over time by electrode cluster and randomization status (p = 0.633, partial η2 = 0.02).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Intervention effects on child development measured by the BSID-III (Intervention 35, control 36). (A) Generalized estimating equations models results had the following parameters for each child development domain: cognitive development (coefficient = 0.46, 95% CI [0.85, 1.79], p = 0.488), receptive language development (coefficient = 0.44, 95% CI [− 0.32, 1.19], p = 0.255), expressive language development (coefficient = 0.89, 95% CI [0.18, 1.61], p = 0.014), fine motor development (coefficient = 0.23, 95% CI [− 0.56, 1.04], p = 0.561), gross motor development (coefficient = 1.32, 95% CI [− 0.55, 3.20], p = 0.166). (B) Fitted models plots of all child development domains over time by randomization status. Standardized effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated from endpoint marginal means: cognitive development (d = 0.08), receptive language development (d = 0.17), expressive language development (d = 0.37), fine motor development (d = 0.05), gross motor development (d = 0.21).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Intervention effects on anthropometric development (BMI-for-age: intervention n = 36, control n = 39; Length-for-age: intervention n = 35, control n = 37). (A) Generalized estimating equations models results had the following parameters for each child anthropometric development measure: BMI-for-age (coefficient = − 0.05, 95% CI [− 0.44, 0.34], p = 0.800), length-for-age (coefficient = 0.10, 95% CI [− 0.27, 0.49], p = 0.583). (B) Fitted models plots of all child development domains over time by randomization status. Standardized effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated from endpoint marginal means: BMI-for-age (d = − 0.05), length-for-age (d = 0.10).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Intervention effects on mother–child relationship and home environment measured by the HOME (intervention n = 30, control n = 32). (A) Generalized estimating equations models results had the following parameters for each HOME domain: emotional/verbal responsivity (coefficient = 0.97, 95% CI [0.37, 1.58], p = 0.002), avoidance of restriction and punishment (coefficient = 0.18, 95% CI [− 0.22, 0.59], p = 0.372), organization of the physical/temporal environment (coefficient = 0.14, 95% CI [− 0.20, 0.49], p = 0.406), provision of appropriate play materials (coefficient = 0.14, 95% CI [– 0.54, 0.82], p = 0.687), parental involvement with the child (coefficient = 0.28, 95% CI [– 0.06, 0.64], p = 0.110), opportunities for variety in daily stimulation (coefficient = 0.37, 95% CI [0.09, 0.66], p = 0.009). (B) Fitted models plots of all HOME domains over time by randomization status. Standardized effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated from endpoint marginal means: emotional/verbal responsivity (d = 0.55), avoidance of restriction and punishment (d = 0.16), organization of the physical/temporal environment (d = 0.16), provision of appropriate play materials (d = 0.07), parental involvement with the child (d = 0.27), opportunities for variety in daily stimulation (d = 0.46).

References

    1. Black MM, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: Science through the life course. Lancet. 2017;389:77–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7.
    1. Britto PR, et al. Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389:91–102. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3.
    1. Grantham-McGregor S, et al. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:60–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60032-4.
    1. Campbell F, et al. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014;343:1478–1485. doi: 10.1126/science.1248429.
    1. Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Ou S-R, Arteaga IA, White BAB. School-based early childhood education and age-28 well-being: Effects by timing, dosage, and subgroups. Science. 2011;333:360–364. doi: 10.1126/science.1203618.
    1. Schweinhart, L. J. et al. The High/scope perry preschool study through age 40: Summary, conclusions, and frequently asked questions. (2005).
    1. Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2010;20:327–348. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4.
    1. Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: Life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2005;7:103–123. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2005.7.2/mmeaney.
    1. Gao W, et al. A review on neuroimaging studies of genetic and environmental influences on early brain development. Neuroimage. 2019;185:802–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.032.
    1. Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 2004;59:77–92. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77.
    1. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233.
    1. Jensen SKG, Berens AE, Nelson CA., 3rd Effects of poverty on interacting biological systems underlying child development. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2017;1:225–239. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30024-X.
    1. Braveman P, Barclay C. Health disparities beginning in childhood: A life-course perspective. Pediatrics. 2009;124:S163–S175. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1100D.
    1. Caspi A, et al. Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2016;1:0005. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0005.
    1. Brahmbhatt H, et al. Prevalence and determinants of adolescent pregnancy in urban disadvantaged settings across five cities. J. Adolesc. Health. 2014;55:S48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.07.023.
    1. Ganchimeg T, et al. Pregnancy and childbirth outcomes among adolescent mothers: A World Health Organization multicountry study. BJOG. 2014;121(Suppl 1):40–48. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.12630.
    1. Firk C, Konrad K, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Scharke W, Dahmen B. Cognitive development in children of adolescent mothers: The impact of socioeconomic risk and maternal sensitivity. Infant Behav. Dev. 2018;50:238–246. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.02.002.
    1. D’Onofrio BM, et al. Maternal age at childbirth and offspring disruptive behaviors: Testing the causal hypothesis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2009;50:1018–1028. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02068.x.
    1. Anselmi L, Piccinini CA, Barros FC, Lopes RS. Psychosocial determinants of behaviour problems in Brazilian preschool children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2004;45:779–788. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00271.x.
    1. de Borba LS, Valentini NC. Motor and cognitive development of infants of adolescent and adult mothers: Longitudinal study. Rev. Bras. Cineantropom. Desempenho Hum. 2015;17:438–449. doi: 10.5007/1980-0037.2015v17n4p438.
    1. Cone JN, Hendrick CE, Owotomo O, Al-Hamoodah L, Maslowsky J. Socioeconomic well-being in early adulthood among repeat versus one-time teenage mothers. Youth Soc. 2019 doi: 10.1177/0044118X19892455.
    1. Nunes AP, Phipps MG. Postpartum depression in adolescent and adult mothers: Comparing prenatal risk factors and predictive models. Matern. Child Health J. 2013;17:1071–1079. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-1089-5.
    1. Bernard K, Nissim G, Vaccaro S, Harris JL, Lindhiem O. Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: A meta-analysis. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2018;20:578–599. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1430839.
    1. RivaCrugnola C, Ierardi E, Gazzotti S, Albizzati A. Motherhood in adolescent mothers: Maternal attachment, mother–infant styles of interaction and emotion regulation at three months. Infant Behav. Dev. 2014;37:44–56. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.011.
    1. Grau, J. M., Wilson, K. S., Smith, E. N., Castellanos, P. & Duran, P. A. Adolescent Parenting: Risk and Protective Factors in the Context of Poverty. in The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development (ed. Rosalind King And) (Maholmes, V & King, RB, 2012). 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199769100.013.0009.
    1. Ferraro AA, et al. Childbearing in adolescence: intergenerational dejà-vu? Evidence from a Brazilian birth cohort. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2013;13:149. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-13-149.
    1. Clark H, et al. A future for the world’s children? A WHO-UNICEF-Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;395:605–658. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)32540-1.
    1. Doyle O, Harmon CP, Heckman JJ, Tremblay RE. Investing in early human development: Timing and economic efficiency. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2009;7:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.002.
    1. Lindsay KL, Buss C, Wadhwa PD, Entringer S. The interplay between nutrition and stress in pregnancy: Implications for fetal programming of brain development. Biol. Psychiatry. 2019;85:135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.06.021.
    1. Jansson LM, Jordan CJ, Velez ML. Perinatal marijuana use and the developing child. JAMA. 2018;320:545–546. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.8401.
    1. Mbuagbaw, L. et al. Health system and community level interventions for improving antenatal care coverage and health outcomes. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.12 (2015).
    1. Walker SP, et al. Child development: Risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2.
    1. Levenstein P, Levenstein S, Oliver D. First grade school readiness of former child participants in a South Carolina replication of the Parent–Child Home Program. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2002;23:331–353. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00112-0.
    1. Gertler P, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344:998–1001. doi: 10.1126/science.1251178.
    1. Walker SP, Chang SM, Vera-Hernández M, Grantham-McGregor S. Early childhood stimulation benefits adult competence and reduces violent behavior. Pediatrics. 2011;127:849–857. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2231.
    1. Olds DL. The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Ment. Health J. 2006;27:5–25. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20077.
    1. Olds DL, et al. Effects of nurse home-visiting on maternal life course and child development: Age 6 follow-up results of a randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2004;114:1550–1559. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0962.
    1. Mejdoubi J, et al. The effect of VoorZorg, the Dutch nurse–family partnership, on child maltreatment and development: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0120182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120182.
    1. Mejdoubi J, et al. Effect of nurse home visits vs. usual care on reducing intimate partner violence in young high-risk pregnant women: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2013;8:e78185. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078185.
    1. Olds DL, et al. Effect of home visiting by nurses on maternal and child mortality: Results of a 2-decade follow-up of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168:800–806. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.472.
    1. Darroch, J. E., Woog, V., Bankole, A. & Ashford, L. S. Costs and benefits of meeting the contraceptive needs of adolescents. (Guttmacher Institute, 2016).
    1. World Health Organization. Adolescent pregnancy. (2018).
    1. United Nations Population Fund. Girlhood, Not Motherhood: Preventing Adolescent Pregnancy. (2015).
    1. McCoy DC, et al. Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: National, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13:e1002034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034.
    1. Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa (ABEP). Critério de Classificação Econômica: Critério Brasil 2015 e atualização da distribuição de classes para 2016. (2015).
    1. Fundação Sistema Estadual de Análise de Dados (SEADE). Portal de Estatísticas do Estado de São Paulo. (2018).
    1. Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 1988;8:77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5.
    1. Richter P, Werner J, Heerlein A, Kraus A, Sauer H. On the validity of the beck depression inventory. Psychopathology. 1998;31:160–168. doi: 10.1159/000066239.
    1. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893.
    1. Kessler RC, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 2005;35:245–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892.
    1. Gorenstein C, Andrade L. Validation of a Portuguese version of the Beck Depression Inventory and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory in Brazilian subjects. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 1996;29:453–457.
    1. Cunha, J. A. Manual da versão em português das escalas Beck. (2001).
    1. Polanczyk G, et al. ADHD in a representative sample of the Brazilian population: Estimated prevalence and comparative adequacy of criteria between adolescents and adults according to the item response theory. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2010;19:177–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.319.
    1. Bernstein DP, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:169–190. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0.
    1. Grassi-Oliveira R, et al. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) in Brazilian samples of different age groups: Findings from confirmatory factor analysis. PLoS One. 2014;9:e87118. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087118.
    1. Grassi-Oliveira R, Stein LM, Pezzi JC. Tradução e validação de conteúdo da versão em português do Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Rev. Saúde Pública. 2006;40:249–255. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102006000200010.
    1. dos Santos LP, et al. Proposal of a short-form version of the Brazilian Food Insecurity Scale. Rev. Saúde Pública. 2014;48:783–789. doi: 10.1590/S0034-8910.2014048005195.
    1. Corrêa AMS. Food insecurity as measured by individual perceptions. Estud. Av. 2007;21:143–154. doi: 10.1590/S0103-40142007000200012.
    1. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry. 1982;52:664–678. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x.
    1. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977;84:191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191.
    1. Bronfenbrenner U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Dev. Psychol. 1986;22:723–742. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723.
    1. Chiesa AM, et al. A construção de tecnologias de atenção em saúde com base na promoção da saúde. Rev. Esc. Enferm. USP. 2009;43:1352–1357. doi: 10.1590/S0080-62342009000600036.
    1. Martins, J., Veríssimo, M. D. L. Ó. R. & Oliveira, M. A. Avaliação dos instrumentos do projeto “Nossas Crianças: Janelas de Oportunidades”, segundo agentes comunitários de saúde. Texto Contexto Enferm.17, 106–114 (2008).
    1. Sadler LS, et al. Minding the Baby: Enhancing reflectiveness to improve early health and relationship outcomes in an interdisciplinary home visiting program. Infant Ment. Health J. 2013;34:391–405. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21406.
    1. Pinheiro DGM, et al. Validação de conteúdo do programa de visita domiciliar para a díade mãe-criança. Cogitare Enfermagem. 2018;23:e54055. doi: 10.5380/ce.v23i2.54055.
    1. Fracolli LA, de Reticena KO, de Abreu FCP, Chiesa AM. A implementação de um programa de visitas domiciliárias com foco na parentalidade: um relato de experiência. Rev. Esc. Enferm. USP. 2018;52:e03361. doi: 10.1590/s1980-220x2017044003361.
    1. Siqueira L, et al. Nurse home visit program to promote parenting in Brazil: Premises and planning. Rev. Esc. Enferm. USP. 2020 doi: 10.21203/-34304/v1.
    1. Paim J, Travassos C, Almeida C, Bahia L, Macinko J. The Brazilian health system: History, advances, and challenges. Lancet. 2011;377:1778–1797. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60054-8.
    1. Brasil. Ministério da Saúde. Gestação de Alto Risco. (2010).
    1. Brasil. Ministério da Saúde. Caderneta da Gestante. (2018).
    1. Brasil. Ministério da Saúde. Atenção ao pré-natal de baixo risco. Cadernos de Atenção Básica (2012).
    1. McLaughlin KA, et al. Delayed maturation in brain electrical activity partially explains the association between early environmental deprivation and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;68:329–336. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.005.
    1. Bayley, N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development, Third Edition. (Pearson, 2005).
    1. Viana TP, de Andrade ISN, Lopes ANM. Desenvolvimento cognitivo e linguagem em prematuros. Audiol. Commun. Res. 2014;19:1–6. doi: 10.1590/S2317-64312014000100002.
    1. Madaschi V, Mecca TP, Macedo EC, Paula CS. Bayley-III scales of infant and toddler development: Transcultural adaptation and psychometric properties. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto) 2016;26:189–197. doi: 10.1590/1982-43272664201606.
    1. de Freitas M, et al. Follow-up of premature children with high risk for growth and development delay: A multiprofessional assessment. Einstein. 2010;8:180–186. doi: 10.1590/s1679-45082010ao1569.
    1. World Health Organization. WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: Methods and development. (2006).
    1. World Health Organization. WHO | WHO Anthro (version 3.2.2, January 2011) and macros. (2011).
    1. Caldwell, B. M. & Bradley, R. H. Home observation for measurement of the environment. (University of Arkansas at Little Rock Little Rock, 1984).
    1. Totsika V, Sylva K. The home observation for measurement of the environment revisited. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health. 2004;9:25–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1475-357X.2003.00073.x.
    1. de Oliveira EA, Barros FC, da Silva Anselmi LD, Piccinini CA. The Quality of home environment in Brazil: An ecological model. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2006;15:631–642. doi: 10.1007/s10826-006-9063-8.
    1. Lima MC, et al. Determinants of mental and motor development at 12 months in a low income population: a cohort study in northeast Brazil. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:969–975. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2004.tb18257.x.
    1. Andrade SA, et al. Family environment and child’s cognitive development: An epidemiological approach. Rev. Saúde Pública. 2005;39:606–611. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102005000400014.
    1. Liang K-Y, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73:13–22. doi: 10.1093/biomet/73.1.13.
    1. Twisk, J. W. R. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology: A practical guide. (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2013).
    1. Cui J. QIC program and model selection in GEE analyses. STATA Journal. 2013;7:334–350.
    1. Cumming, G. Cohen’d. in Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis (Routledge, 2011).
    1. University of Cambridge Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit. Rules of thumb on magnitudes of effect sizes. FAQ/effectSize - CBU statistics Wiki (2021).
    1. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). Purchasing power parities (PPP) (indicator) 2017. OECD (2017).
    1. Olds DL, et al. Home visiting by paraprofessionals and by nurses: A randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2002;110:486–496. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.3.486.
    1. Longobardi, E. Funzioni comunicative materne nel secondo anno di vita del bambino: come varia il supporto materno in relazione allo sviluppo linguistico. (1995).
    1. Longobardi E, Spataro P, Colonnesi C. Maternal communicative functions and mind-mindedness at 16 months as predictors of children’s internal and non-internal language at 20 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 2018;50:52–63. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.11.003.
    1. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L. Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Dev. 2001;72:748–767. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00313.
    1. Fatori D, et al. Maternal parenting electronic diary in the context of a home visit intervention for adolescent mothers in an urban deprived area of São Paulo, Brazil: Randomized controlled trial. JMIR mHealth uHealth. 2020;8:e13686. doi: 10.2196/13686.
    1. Sheridan MA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, McLaughlin KA, Nelson CA., 3rd Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2012;109:12927–12932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200041109.
    1. Gonçalves H, et al. Infant nutrition and growth: Trends and inequalities in four population-based birth cohorts in Pelotas, Brazil, 1982–2015. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2019;48:i80–i88. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyy233.
    1. Victora CG, et al. Maternal and child health in Brazil: Progress and challenges. Lancet. 2011;377:1863–1876. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60138-4.
    1. Barreto ML, Barros F, Victora C. Saúde reprodutiva, materna, neonatal e infantil nos 30 anos do Sistema Único de Saúde (SUS) Ciência Saúde Coletiva. 2018;23:1915–1928. doi: 10.1590/1413-81232018233.12132016.
    1. Panter-Brick C, et al. Practitioner review: Engaging fathers—recommendations for a game change in parenting interventions based on a systematic review of the global evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014;55:1187–1212. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12280.
    1. Nelson KE, Welsh JA, Trup EMV, Greenberg MT. Language delays of impoverished preschool children in relation to early academic and emotion recognition skills. First Lang. 2011;31:164–194. doi: 10.1177/0142723710391887.
    1. Snowling M, Bishop DV, Stothard SE. Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2000;41:587–600. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00651.
    1. Prior M, Bavin EL, Ong B. Predictors of school readiness in five- to six-year-old children from an Australian longitudinal community sample. Educ. Psychol. 2011;31:3–16. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.541048.
    1. Girard L-C, Pingault J-B, Doyle O, Falissard B, Tremblay RE. Expressive language and prosocial behaviour in early childhood: Longitudinal associations in the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2017;14:381–398. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1215300.
    1. Hay DF. Prosocial development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1994;35:29–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01132.x.
    1. Hay DF, Payne A, Chadwick A. Peer relations in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2004;45:84–108. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x.
    1. Olds DL, et al. Prenatal and infancy nurse home visiting effects on mothers: 18-Year follow-up of a randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2019;144:e20183889. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-3889.
    1. Eckenrode J, et al. Long-term effects of prenatal and infancy nurse home visitation on the life course of youths: 19-year follow-up of a randomized trial. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2010;164:9–15. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.240.

Source: PubMed

3
Suscribir