Children's Utilization of Toys is Moderated by Age-Appropriateness, Toy Category, and Child Age

Melissa N Richards, Diane L Putnick, Laura P Bradley, Kyle M Lang, Todd D Little, Joan T D Suwalsky, Marc H Bornstein, Melissa N Richards, Diane L Putnick, Laura P Bradley, Kyle M Lang, Todd D Little, Joan T D Suwalsky, Marc H Bornstein

Abstract

Play during childhood is essential to growth and learning. Little is known about whether categories of toys moderate play behaviors at different ages, or how children interact with toys that are simple, appropriate, or complex for their developmental level. Two hundred and forty-three children between the ages of 1 and 8 years, divided into four age groups, played with toys that were targeted to their age group as well as toys aimed at one age group younger and older. Toys fell into nine different categories. Whether children fully utilized each toy was evaluated. Analyses examined how children's utilization of toys was affected by the age-appropriateness of the toy, the category of toy, and the child's age. Considering all age groups and toys, children were less likely to fully utilize toys targeted toward older children than age-appropriate toys, but this effect was moderated by the toy category and the child's age.

Keywords: age-appropriateness; play complexity; play scale; toy play.

References

    1. ASTM Standard F963 (2011). Standard Consumer Safety Specification for Toy Safety, (1986). ASTM International, West Conshohocken, PA, 2003, .
    1. Bergen D (2002). The role of pretend play in children’s cognitive development. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 4, 1–13.
    1. Bornstein MH (2007). On the significance of social relationships in the development of children’s earliest symbolic play: An ecological perspective. In Gönçü A & Gaskins S (Eds.), Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives (pp. 101–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
    1. Bornstein MH, DiPietro JA, Hahn C, Painter KM, Haynes OM, & Costigan KA (2002). Prenatal cardiac function and postnatal cognitive development: An exploratory study. Infancy, 3, 475–494. 10.1207/S15327078IN0304_04
    1. Bornstein MH, Selmi AM, Haynes OM, Painter KM, & Marx ES (1999). Representational abilities and the hearing status of child/mother dyads. Child Development, 70, 833–852. 10.1111/1467-8624.00060
    1. Corter C, & Jamieson N (1977). Infants’ toy preferences and mothers’ predictions. Developmental Psychology, 13, 413–414. 10.1037/0012-1649.13.4.413
    1. Crawley SB, & Sherrod KB (1984). Parent-infant play during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 65–75.
    1. Enders CK (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
    1. Fenson L, Kagan J, Kearsley RB, & Zelazo PR (1976). The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. Child Development, 47, 232–236.
    1. Fisher KR, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, & Gryfe SG (2008). Conceptual split? Parents’ and experts; perceptions of play in the 21st century. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 305–316.
    1. Gallup (2017). Time to play-- A study on children’s free time: How it is spent, prioritized, and valued. Washington, DC: Author.
    1. Ginsburg KR (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics, 119, 182–191. 10.1542/peds.2006-2697
    1. Glassy D & Romano J (2003). Selecting appropriate toys for young children: The pediatrician’s role. Pediatrics, 111, 911–913. 10.1542/peds.111.4.911
    1. Graham JW, Olchowski AE, & Gilreath TD (2007). How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prevention Science, 8, 206–213. 10.1007/s11121-007-0070-9
    1. Graham JW, Taylor BJ, Olchowski AE, & Cumsille PE (2006). Planned missing data designs in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 11, 323–343. 10.1037/1082-989X.11.4.323
    1. Hastie T, Tibshirani R, & Friedman J (2009). The elements of statistical learning (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Springer. 10.1007/978-0-387-84858-7
    1. Howard W, Rhemtulla M, & Little TD (2015). Using principal components as auxiliary variables in missing data estimation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50, 285–299. 10.1080/00273171.2014.999267
    1. IBM. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0 [Computer software]. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
    1. Kaugars AS, & Russ SW (2009). Assessing preschool children’s pretend play: Preliminary validation of the Affect in Play Scale—Preschool version. Early Education and Development, 201, 733–755. 10.1080/10409280802545388
    1. Kimmerle M, Mick LA, & Michel GF (1995). Bimanual role-differentiated toy play during infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 299–307.
    1. Lang KM, & Little TD (2016). Principled missing data treatments. Prevention Science. Advanced online publication. 10.1007/s11121-016-0644-5
    1. Lang KM, Little TD, & PcAux Development Team (2017). PcAux: Automatically extract auxiliary features for simple, principled missing data analysis (R package version 0.0.0.9004). Retrieved from
    1. Lillard AS (2015). The development of play. In Lerner RM (Ed.), The Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (pp. 425–468). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy211
    1. Lindsey EW, & Mize J (2001). Contextual differences in parent–child play: Implications for children’s gender role development. Sex Roles, 44, 155–176.
    1. Little TD, & Rhemtulla M (2013). Planned missing data designs for developmental researchers. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 199–204. doi:10.111/cdep.12043
    1. Little TD, Jorgensen TD, Lang KM, & Moore EWG (2013). On the joys of missing data. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39, 1–12. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsto48
    1. MacDonald K, & Parke RD (1986). Parent-child physical play: The effects of sex and age of children and parents. Sex Roles, 15, 367–378.
    1. Olszewski P, & Fuson KC (1982). Verbally expressed fantasy play of preschoolers as a function of toy structure. Developmental Psychology, 18, 57–61.
    1. Piaget J (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
    1. Power TG (1985). Mother-and father-infant play: A developmental analysis. Child Development, 56, 1514–1524.
    1. Power TG, & Parke RD (1983). Patterns of mother and father play with their 8-month-old infant: A multiple analyses approach. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 453–459.
    1. Putnam SP, & Rothbart MK (2006). Development of Short and Very Short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 103–113. 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09
    1. Putnam SP, Jacobs J, Gartstein MA, & Rothbart MK (2010, March). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Poster presented at International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD.
    1. R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from
    1. Robinson CC & Jackson R (1987). The effects of varying toy detail within a prototypical play object on the solitary pretend play of preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8, 209–220. 10.1016/0193-3973(87)90015-3
    1. Russ SW (2014). Pretend play in childhood. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
    1. Scarlett WG (2005). Children’s play. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.
    1. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA (2005). Vineland-II: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson.
    1. Tamis-LeMonda CS & Bornstein MH (1991). Individual variation, correspondence, stability, and change in mother and toddler play. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 143–162. 10.1016/0163-6383(91)90002-A
    1. The Small Parts Regulation, 16 C.F.R. §1501 (1979). U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission. Bethesda, MD.
    1. Toy Industry Association (2017). Annual Sales Data. Retrieved from
    1. Trawick-Smith J & Dzuirgot T (2011). “Good-fit” teacher-child play interactions and subsequent autonomous play in preschool. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 110–123. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.04.005
    1. Tu Y (2012). Toy-related deaths and injuries: Calendar year 2012. Directorate for Epidemiology, U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, Washington, DC.
    1. Van Buuren S (2012). Flexible imputation of missing data. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. 10.1201/b11826
    1. Van Buuren S, Brand JPL, Groothuis-Oudshoorn CGM, & Rubin DB (2006). Fully conditional specification in multivariate imputation. Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation, 76, 1049–1064. 10.1080/10629360600810434
    1. Vygotsky LS (1967). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 5, 6–18. 10.2753/RPO1061-040505036
    1. Vygotsky LS, (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    1. Wolfgang CH, Stannard LL, & Jones I (2001). Play performance among preschoolers as a predictor of later school achievement in mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 15, 173–180. 10.1080/02568540109594958

Source: PubMed

3
Suscribir