Birth weight and catch up growth are associated with childhood impulsivity in two independent cohorts

Patrícia P Silveira, Irina Pokhvisneva, Hélène Gaudreau, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Birit F P Broekman, Meir Steiner, Robert Levitan, Carine Parent, Josie Diorio, Michael J Meaney, Patrícia P Silveira, Irina Pokhvisneva, Hélène Gaudreau, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Birit F P Broekman, Meir Steiner, Robert Levitan, Carine Parent, Josie Diorio, Michael J Meaney

Abstract

Individuals born after intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) are more impulsive towards palatable foods, but it is not clear 1) if IUGR-related impulsivity is specific for foods and solely based on response inhibition and 2) if the development of impulsivity is due to being born IUGR per se or to growing up fast in the first few years of life (catch up growth). Children were classified in the IUGR group if the birth weight ratio was below 0.85. Delta z score for BMI was used as a measure of catch up growth. In MAVAN (N = 274), impulsivity was measured by the Information Sampling Task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (IST - CANTAB), and in GUSTO using the Sticker Delay Task (N = 327). There is a significant effect of interaction between being born IUGR and the magnitude of catch up growth on the reflection impulsivity from IST-CANTAB at 60 months, in which greater catch up growth associates with greater impulsivity in the IST fixed condition in IUGR children. The finding was reproduced in children from the GUSTO cohort using the Sticker Delay Task. We confirmed that catch up growth interacts with IUGR, having a major role in the development of impulsivity in the first years of life and influencing inhibitory control and decision making processes.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Information Sampling Task. (a) Initial screen. The child is instructed to click on the gray boxes to open them, and she can open as many as she wants before deciding which color comprises the majority of the 25 boxes; (b) when the child decided that she had enough information to choose which color was more prevalent, she should click on the corresponding color at the bottom; (c) all boxes then automatically opened and a message about being correct or incorrect was displayed before the next trial.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Information Sampling Task (CANTAB at 60 months of age, MAVAN) - mean P correct score, win condition fixed. The lower the score depicted in the y axis, the more impulsive is the child. There is an interaction between being born IUGR (enclosed dots) and variation in BMI z-scores over the first 5 years of life on impulsivity scores, in which greater catch up growth is related to poorer scores (increased reflection impulsivity) in IUGR children.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Sticker Delay Task (41 months of age, GUSTO). The lower the score depicted in the y axis, the more impulsive is the child. There is an interaction between being born IUGR (enclosed dots) and variation in BMI z-scores over the first 4 years of life on impulsivity scores, in which greater catch up growth is related to poorer scores (increased impulsivity) in IUGR children.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Theoretical framework, life-course upwards spiral (follow the spiral from bottom to top). Please see details in the Discussion section. DA = dopamine.

References

    1. Hales CN, Barker DJ. Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia. 1992;35:595–601. doi: 10.1007/BF00400248.
    1. Barker DJ, et al. Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus, hypertension and hyperlipidaemia (syndrome X): relation to reduced fetal growth. Diabetologia. 1993;36:62–67. doi: 10.1007/BF00399095.
    1. Lahti, M. et al. Late preterm birth, post-term birth, and abnormal fetal growth as risk factors for severe mental disorders from early to late adulthood. Psychol Med, 1–15, 10.1017/S0033291714001998 (2014).
    1. Silveira PP, et al. Preliminary evidence for an impulsivity-based thrifty eating phenotype. Pediatric research. 2012;71:293–298. doi: 10.1038/pr.2011.39.
    1. Reis RS, et al. Poor infant inhibitory control predicts food fussiness in childhood - A possible protective role of n-3 PUFAs for vulnerable children. Prostaglandins, leukotrienes, and essential fatty acids. 2015;97:21–25. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2015.03.004.
    1. Reis RS, et al. Impulsivity-based thrifty eating phenotype and the protective role of n-3 PUFAs intake in adolescents. Translational psychiatry. 2016;6:e755. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.16.
    1. Barbieri MA, et al. Severe intrauterine growth restriction is associated with higher spontaneous carbohydrate intake in young women. Pediatric research. 2009;65:215–220. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31818d6850.
    1. Migraine A, et al. Effect of preterm birth and birth weight on eating behavior at 2 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:1270–1277. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.051151.
    1. Portella AK, Silveira PP. Neurobehavioral determinants of nutritional security in fetal growth-restricted individuals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2014
    1. Clark L, Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Sahakian BJ. Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:515–522. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.007.
    1. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2014;18:177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003.
    1. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;146:348–361. doi: 10.1007/PL00005481.
    1. Dougherty DM, et al. Behavioral impulsivity paradigms: a comparison in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2003;44:1145–1157. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00197.
    1. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. The American journal of psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783.
    1. Kagan J. Reflection–impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1966;71:17–24. doi: 10.1037/h0022886.
    1. Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Fattal-Valevski A, Harel S. Memory functions of children born with asymmetric intrauterine growth restriction. Brain research. 2006;1117:186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.004.
    1. Pryor J, Silva PA, Brooke M. Growth, Development and Behavior in Adolescents Born Small-for-Gestational-Age. Journal of paediatrics and child health. 1995;31:403–407. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1995.tb00847.x.
    1. Phua DY, Rifkin-Graboi A, Saw SM, Meaney MJ, Qiu A. Executive functions of six-year-old boys with normal birth weight and gestational age. PloS one. 2012;7:e36502. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036502.
    1. Berends LM, Fernandez-Twinn DS, Martin-Gronert MS, Cripps RL, Ozanne SE. Catch-up growth following intra-uterine growth-restriction programmes an insulin-resistant phenotype in adipose tissue. International journal of obesity. 2013;37:1051–1057. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.196.
    1. Milovanovic I, et al. SGA children with moderate catch-up growth are showing the impaired insulin secretion at the age of 4. PloS one. 2014;9:e100337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100337.
    1. Soto N, et al. Insulin sensitivity and secretion are related to catch-up growth in small-for-gestational-age infants at age 1 year: results from a prospective cohort. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2003;88:3645–3650. doi: 10.1210/jc.2002-030031.
    1. Monfils Gustafsson W, Josefsson A, Ekholm Selling K, Sydsjo G. Preterm birth or foetal growth impairment and psychiatric hospitalization in adolescence and early adulthood in a Swedish population-based birth cohort. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2009;119:54–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01267.x.
    1. Class QA, Rickert ME, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM. Fetal growth and psychiatric and socioeconomic problems: population-based sibling comparison. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science. 2014;205:355–361. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.143693.
    1. Pettersson E, et al. Birth weight as an independent predictor of ADHD symptoms: a within-twin pair analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2015;56:453–459. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12299.
    1. O’Donnell KG, et al. The Maternal Adversity Vulnerability and Neurodevelopment (MAVAN) Project: Theory and methodology. Can J Psychiatry. 2014;59:497–508. doi: 10.1177/070674371405900906.
    1. Kramer MS, et al. A new and improved population-based Canadian reference for birth weight for gestational age. Pediatrics. 2001;108:E35. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.2.e35.
    1. Kramer MS, Platt R, Yang H, McNamara H, Usher RH. Are all growth-restricted newborns created equal(ly)? Pediatrics. 1999;103:599–602. doi: 10.1542/peds.103.3.599.
    1. WHO. (ed World Health Organization) 312 (Geneva, 2006).
    1. Villar J, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the Newborn Cross-Sectional Study of the INTERGROWTH-21st Project. Lancet. 2014;384:857–868. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60932-6.
    1. Clark L, Roiser JP, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Disrupted ‘reflection’ impulsivity in cannabis users but not current or former ecstasy users. J Psychopharmacol. 2009;23:14–22. doi: 10.1177/0269881108089587.
    1. Bennett, D. et al. Systematic Overestimation of Reflection Impulsivity in the Information Sampling Task. Biol Psychiatry, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.027 (2016).
    1. Clark, L. & Robbins, T. W. Reply to: Systematic Overestimation of Reflection Impulsivity in the Information Sampling Task. Biol Psychiatry, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.018 (2016).
    1. Pokhvisneva, I., Leger, E., Meaney, M. J. & Silveira, P. P. Systematic Overestimation of Reflection Impulsivity in the Information Sampling Task: Age Dependency in Children. Biol Psychiatry, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.027 (2017).
    1. Soh SE, et al. Cohort profile: Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO) birth cohort study. International journal of epidemiology. 2014;43:1401–1409. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt125.
    1. Kochanska G. Socialization and temperament in the development of guilt and conscience. Child development. 1991;62:1379–1392. doi: 10.2307/1130813.
    1. Solowij N, et al. Reflection impulsivity in adolescent cannabis users: a comparison with alcohol-using and non-substance-using adolescents. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;219:575–586. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2486-y.
    1. Crume, T. L. et al. The Long-term impact of intrauterine growth restriction in a diverse US cohort of children: The EPOCH study. Obesity (Silver Spring), 10.1002/oby.20565 (2013).
    1. Lussana F, et al. Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine is associated with a preference for fatty foods and a more atherogenic lipid profile. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88:1648–1652. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26140.
    1. Bischoff AR, et al. Low birth weight is associated with increased fat intake in school-aged boys. The British journal of nutrition. 2018;119:1295–1302. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518000892.
    1. Hawkins MA, Gunstad J, Calvo D, Spitznagel MB. Higher fasting glucose is associated with poorer cognition among healthy young adults. Health Psychol. 2016;35:199–202. doi: 10.1037/hea0000248.
    1. Ojala KP, Tiihonen J, Repo-Tiihonen E, Tikkanen R, Virkkunen M. Basal insulin secretion, PCL-R and recidivism among impulsive violent alcoholic offenders. Psychiatry Res. 2015;225:420–424. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.073.
    1. Alves MB, Dalle Molle R, Desai M, Ross MG, Silveira PP. Increased palatable food intake and response to food cues in intrauterine growth-restricted rats are related to tyrosine hydroxylase content in the orbitofrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. Behavioural brain research. 2015;287:73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.019.
    1. Yang S, et al. Pre-natal and post-natal growth trajectories and childhood cognitive ability and mental health. International journal of epidemiology. 2011;40:1215–1226. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr094.
    1. Sammallahti S, et al. Infant growth after preterm birth and neurocognitive abilities in young adulthood. The Journal of pediatrics. 2014;165:1109–1115 e1103. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.08.028.
    1. Sammallahti S, et al. Growth after late-preterm birth and adult cognitive, academic, and mental health outcomes. Pediatric research. 2017;81:767–774. doi: 10.1038/pr.2016.276.
    1. Cardoso, M. A. et al. Post-term growth and cognitive development at 5 years of age in preterm children: Evidence from a prospective population-based cohort. PloS one12, 10.1371/journal.pone.0174645 (2017).
    1. O’Donnell KJ, Meaney MJ. Fetal Origins of Mental Health: The Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Hypothesis. The American journal of psychiatry. 2017;174:319–328. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020138.
    1. Garofano A, Czernichow P, Breant B. In utero undernutrition impairs rat beta-cell development. Diabetologia. 1997;40:1231–1234. doi: 10.1007/s001250050812.
    1. Mericq V, et al. Longitudinal changes in insulin sensitivity and secretion from birth to age three years in small- and appropriate-for-gestational-age children. Diabetologia. 2005;48:2609–2614. doi: 10.1007/s00125-005-0036-z.
    1. Laureano DP, et al. Intrauterine growth restriction modifies the hedonic response to sweet taste in newborn pups - Role of the accumbal mu-opioid receptors. Neuroscience. 2016;322:500–508. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.033.
    1. Li C, McDonald TJ, Wu GY, Nijland MJ, Nathanielsz PW. Intrauterine growth restriction alters term fetal baboon hypothalamic appetitive peptide balance. J Endocrinol. 2013;217:275–282. doi: 10.1530/JOE-13-0012.
    1. Barker DJ. The developmental origins of well-being. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences. 2004;359:1359–1366. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1518.
    1. Ayres C, et al. Intrauterine growth restriction and the fetal programming of the hedonic response to sweet taste in newborn infants. International journal of pediatrics. 2012;2012:657379. doi: 10.1155/2012/657379.
    1. Hajnal A, Norgren R. Accumbens dopamine mechanisms in sucrose intake. Brain research. 2001;904:76–84. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02451-9.
    1. Kaseva N, et al. Diet and Nutrient Intake in Young Adults Born Preterm at Very Low Birth Weight. J Pediatr-Us. 2013;163:43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.12.076.
    1. Perala, M. M. et al. Body Size at Birth Is Associated with Food and Nutrient Intake in Adulthood. PloS one7, 10.1371/journal.pone.0046139 (2012).
    1. Castanys-Munoz E, et al. Systematic review indicates postnatal growth in term infants born small-for-gestational-age being associated with later neurocognitive and metabolic outcomes. Acta Paediatr. 2017;106:1230–1238. doi: 10.1111/apa.13868.
    1. Chiavaroli V, et al. Insulin Resistance and Oxidative Stress in Children Born Small and Large for Gestational Age. Pediatrics. 2009;124:695–702. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3056.
    1. Kullmann S, et al. Brain Insulin Resistance at the Crossroads of Metabolic and Cognitive Disorders in Humans. Physiol Rev. 2016;96:1169–1209. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2015.
    1. Blazquez E, Velazquez E, Hurtado-Carneiro V, Ruiz-Albusac JM. Insulin in the brain: its pathophysiological implications for States related with central insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 2014;5:161.
    1. Sears B, Perry M. The role of fatty acids in insulin resistance. Lipids in health and disease. 2015;14:121. doi: 10.1186/s12944-015-0123-1.
    1. Figlewicz DP, Bennett JL, Aliakbari S, Zavosh A, Sipols AJ. Insulin acts at different CNS sites to decrease acute sucrose intake and sucrose self-administration in rats. Am J Physiol-Reg I. 2008;295:R388–R394. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.00178.2008.
    1. Schoffelmeer AN, et al. Insulin modulates cocaine-sensitive monoamine transporter function and impulsive behavior. J Neurosci. 2011;31:1284–1291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3779-10.2011.
    1. Kleinridders A, et al. Insulin resistance in brain alters dopamine turnover and causes behavioral disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:3463–3468. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500877112.
    1. Kemp DE, et al. Use of insulin sensitizers for the treatment of major depressive disorder: A pilot study of pioglitazone for major depression accompanied by abdominal obesity. J Affect Disorders. 2012;136:1164–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.033.
    1. Marazziti D, Rutigliano G, Baroni S, Landi P, Dell’Osso L. Metabolic syndrome and major depression. CNS spectrums. 2014;19:293–304. doi: 10.1017/S1092852913000667.
    1. Hryhorczuk C, Sharma S, Fulton SE. Metabolic disturbances connecting obesity and depression. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013;7:177. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00177.
    1. Stoeckel LE, et al. Complex mechanisms linking neurocognitive dysfunction to insulin resistance and other metabolic dysfunction. F1000Res. 2016;5:353.
    1. Biessels GJ, Reagan LP. Hippocampal insulin resistance and cognitive dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:660–671. doi: 10.1038/nrn4019.
    1. Mucellini AB, Fonseca N, Manfro GG, Silveira PP. Hippocampal insulin resistance and altered food decision-making as players on obesity risk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;77:165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.011.
    1. Maimaiti S, et al. Intranasal Insulin Improves Age-Related Cognitive Deficits and Reverses Electrophysiological Correlates of Brain Aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71:30–39. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu314.
    1. Steculorum SM, Solas M, Bruning JC. The paradox of neuronal insulin action and resistance in the development of aging-associated diseases. Alzheimer’s & dementia: the journal of the Alzheimer’s Association. 2014;10:S3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.12.008.
    1. Stouffer MA, et al. Insulin enhances striatal dopamine release by activating cholinergic interneurons and thereby signals reward. Nature communications. 2015;6:8543. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9543.
    1. Bruce AS, et al. Obese children show hyperactivation to food pictures in brain networks linked to motivation, reward and cognitive control. International journal of obesity. 2010;34:1494–1500. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.84.
    1. Pardey MC, Kumar NN, Goodchild AK, Cornish JL. Catecholamine receptors differentially mediate impulsive choice in the medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex. J Psychopharmacol. 2013;27:203–212. doi: 10.1177/0269881112465497.
    1. Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9984–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007.
    1. Kullmann S, et al. The obese brain: association of body mass index and insulin sensitivity with resting state network functional connectivity. Human brain mapping. 2012;33:1052–1061. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21268.
    1. Ravelli ACJ, et al. Glucose tolerance in adults after prenatal exposure to famine. Lancet. 1998;351:173–177. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)07244-9.
    1. Visentin S, et al. Early origins of adult disease: low birth weight and vascular remodeling. Atherosclerosis. 2014;237:391–399. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2014.09.027.
    1. Giussani M, et al. Cardiovascular risk assessment in children: role of physical activity, family history and parental smoking on BMI and blood pressure. J Hypertens. 2013;31:983–992. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0b013e32835f17c7.
    1. Niederkrotenthaler T, Rasmussen F, Mittendorfer-Rutz E. Perinatal conditions and parental age at birth as risk markers for subsequent suicide attempt and suicide: a population based case-control study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2012;27:729–738. doi: 10.1007/s10654-012-9724-4.
    1. Rani V, Deshmukh R, Jaswal P, Kumar P, Bariwal J. Alzheimer’s disease: Is this a brain specific diabetic condition? Physiology & behavior. 2016;164:259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.041.
    1. Cai Z, Xiao M, Chang L, Yan LJ. Role of insulin resistance in Alzheimer’s disease. Metabolic brain disease. 2015;30:839–851. doi: 10.1007/s11011-014-9631-3.
    1. Verdile G, Fuller SJ, Martins RN. The role of type 2 diabetes in neurodegeneration. Neurobiol Dis. 2015;84:22–38. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.04.008.
    1. Dalle Molle R, et al. Gene and environment interaction: Is the differential susceptibility hypothesis relevant for obesity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;73:326–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.028.
    1. Karlberg J, Albertsson-Wikland K. Growth in full-term small-for-gestational-age infants: from birth to final height. Pediatric research. 1995;38:733–739. doi: 10.1203/00006450-199511000-00017.
    1. Carter, R. C. et al. Fetal Alcohol Growth Restriction and Cognitive Impairment. Pediatrics138, 10.1542/peds.2016-0775 (2016).

Source: PubMed

3
Suscribir