The effects of Cognitive Bias Modification training and oxytocin administration on trust in maternal support: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial

Martine W F T Verhees, Eva Ceulemans, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Marinus H van IJzendoorn, Simon de Winter, Guy Bosmans, Martine W F T Verhees, Eva Ceulemans, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Marinus H van IJzendoorn, Simon de Winter, Guy Bosmans

Abstract

Background: Lack of trust in parental support is a transdiagnostic risk factor for the development of psychological problems throughout the lifespan. Research suggests that children's cognitive attachment representations and related information processing biases could be an important target for interventions aiming to build trust in the parent-child relationship. A paradigm that can alter these biases and increase trust is that of Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM), during which a target processing bias is systematically trained. Trust-related CBM training effects could possibly be enhanced by oxytocin, a neuropeptide that has been proposed to play an important role in social information processing and social relationships. The present article describes the study protocol for a double-blind randomized controlled trial (RCT) aimed at testing the individual and combined effects of CBM training and oxytocin administration on trust in maternal support.

Methods/design: One hundred children (aged 8-12 years) are randomly assigned to one of four intervention conditions. Participants inhale a nasal spray that either contains oxytocin (OT) or a placebo. Additionally, they receive either a CBM training aimed at positively modifying trust-related information processing bias or a neutral placebo training aimed to have no trust-related effects. Main and interaction effects of the interventions are assessed on three levels of trust-related outcome measures: trust-related interpretation bias; self-reported trust; and mother-child interactional behavior. Importantly, side-effects of a single administration of OT in middle childhood are monitored closely to provide further information on the safety of OT administration in this age group.

Discussion: The present RCT is the first study to combine CBM training with oxytocin to test for individual and combined effects on trust in mother. If effective, CBM training and oxytocin could be easily applicable and nonintrusive additions to interventions that target trust in the context of the parent-child relationship.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov, ID: NCT02737254 . Registered on 23 March 2016.

Keywords: Attachment; Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM); Interpretation bias; Middle childhood; Oxytocin; Randomized Controlled Trial (RCT); Trust.

Conflict of interest statement

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study design was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee UZ KU Leuven/Research (S57012) in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent is obtained from each participating mother, informed written assent is obtained from each participating child.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Participant flowchart

References

    1. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969.
    1. Cassidy J, Shaver P. Handbook of attachment: theory, research and clinical applications. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2008.
    1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1978.
    1. Verhage ML, Schuengel C, Madigan S, Fearon RM, Oosterman M, Cassiba R, et al. Narrowing the transmission gap: a synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychol Bull. 2016;142:337–66. doi: 10.1037/bul0000038.
    1. Fearon RP, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Lapsley AM, Roisman GI. The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: a meta‐analytic study. Child Dev. 2010;81:435–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x.
    1. Groh AM, Roisman GI, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ, Fearon R. The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: a meta‐analytic study. Child Dev. 2012;83:591–610.
    1. Madigan S, Brumariu LE, Villani V, Atkinson L, Lyons-Ruth K. Representational and questionnaire measures of attachment: a meta-analysis of relations to child internalizing and externalizing problems. Psychol Bull. 2016;142:367–99. doi: 10.1037/bul0000029.
    1. DeKlyen M, Greenberg MT. Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 637–65.
    1. Egeland B. Attachment-based intervention and prevention programs for young children. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RD, editors. Encyclopedia on early childhood development. Montreal: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2004. pp. 1–7.
    1. Bosmans G, Kerns KA. Attachment in middle childhood: progress and prospects. In: Bosmans G, Kerns KA, editors. Attachment in middle childhood: theoretical advances and new directions in an emerging field, New dir child adolesc dev. 2015. pp. 1–14.
    1. Del Giudice M. Attachment in middle childhood: an evolutionary-developmental perspective. In: Bosmans G, Kerns KA, editors. Attachment in middle childhood: theoretical advances and new directions in an emerging field, New dir child adolesc dev. 2015. pp. 15–30.
    1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychol Bull. 2003;129:195–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195.
    1. Dykas MJ, Cassidy J. Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: theory and evidence. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:19–46. doi: 10.1037/a0021367.
    1. Bosmans G, Braet C, Koster E, De Raedt R. Attachment security and attentional breadth toward the attachment figure in middle childhood. J Clin Child Adolesc. 2009;38:872–82. doi: 10.1080/15374410903258926.
    1. De Winter S, Vandevivere E, Waters TEA, Braet C, Bosmans G. Lack of trust in maternal support is associated with negative interpretations of ambiguous maternal behavior. J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25:146–51. doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0197-4.
    1. Dujardin A, Bosmans G, Braet C, Goossens L. Attachment-related expectations and mother-referent memory bias in middle childhood. Scand J Psychol. 2014;55:296–302. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12129.
    1. Lundahl B, Risser HJ, Lovejoy MC. A meta-analysis of parent training: moderators and follow-up effects. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:86–104. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.004.
    1. Kazdin AE. Conduct disorders in childhood and adolescence. 2. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1995.
    1. Ollendick TH, Greene RW, Austin KE, Fraire MG, Halldorsdottir T, Allen KB, et al. Parent management training and collaborative and proactive solutions: a randomized control trial for oppositional youth. J Clin Child Adolesc. 2016;45:591–604. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1004681.
    1. Mathews A, Mackintosh B. Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:602–15. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.602.
    1. MacLeod C, Koster EH, Fox E. Whither cognitive bias modification research? Commentary on the special section articles. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118:89–99. doi: 10.1037/a0014878.
    1. Hallion LS, Ruscio AM. A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:940–58. doi: 10.1037/a0024355.
    1. De Winter S, Bosmans G, Salemink E. Exploring the causal effect of interpretation bias on attachment expectations. Child Dev. 2017;88:131–40. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12587.
    1. Gordon I, Vander Wyk BC, Bennett RH, Cordeaux C, Lucas MV, Eilbott JA, et al. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:20953–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312857110.
    1. Paloyelis Y, Doyle OM, Zelaya FO, Maltezos S, Williams SC, Fotopoulou A, Howard MA. A spatiotemporal profile of in vivo cerebral blood flow changes following intranasal oxytocin in humans. Biol Psychiat. 2014;79:693–705. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.005.
    1. Riem MME, Van IJzendoorn MH, Tops M, Boksem MAS, Rombouts SARB, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Oxytocin effects on complex brain networks are moderated by experiences of maternal love withdrawal. Eur Neuropsychopharm. 2013;23:1288–95. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.01.011.
    1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e258. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.34.
    1. Quintana DS, Alvares GA, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Do delivery routes of intranasally administered oxytocin account for observed effects on social cognition and behavior? A two-level model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;49:182–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.011.
    1. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005;435:673–6. doi: 10.1038/nature03701.
    1. Buchheim A, Heinrichs M, George C, Pokorny D, Koops E, Henningsen P, et al. Oxytocin enhances the experience of attachment security. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2009;34:1417–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.002.
    1. Dillen C, Bernaerts S, Berra E, Bosmans G, Steyaert J, Alaerts K. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on emotion recognition, social functioning and attachment scales. Vlaams congres kinder- en jeugdpsychiatrie en -psychotherapie. Brussels; Belgium: 2015.
    1. Averbeck BB. Oxytocin and the salience of social cues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:9033–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1004892107.
    1. Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2012;37:438–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.008.
    1. Guastella AJ, MacLeod C. A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. Horm Behav. 2012;61:410–8. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.002.
    1. Owen SF, Tuncdemir SN, Bader PL, Tirko NN, Fishel G, Tsien RW. Oxytocin enhances hippocampal spike transmission by modulating fast-spiking interneurons. Nature. 2013;500:458–62. doi: 10.1038/nature12330.
    1. Dadds MR, MacDonald E, Cauchi A, Williams K, Levy F, Brennan J. Nasal oxytocin for social deficits in childhood autism: a randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:521–31. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1899-3.
    1. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, Hickie IB. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiat. 2010;67:692–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020.
    1. Guastella AJ, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Alvares GA, Tonge BJ, Hickie IB, et al. The effects of a course of intranasal oxytocin on social behaviors in youth diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2015;56:444–52. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12305.
    1. Tachibana M, Kagitani-Shimono K, Mohri I, Yamamoto T, Sanefuji W, Nakamura A, et al. Long-term administration of intranasal oxytocin is a safe and promising therapy for early adolescent boys with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Adol Psychop. 2013;23:123–7. doi: 10.1089/cap.2012.0048.
    1. Taylor AE, Lee H, Buisman-Pijlman FTA. Oxytocin treatment in pediatric populations. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:360. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00360.
    1. Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. The role of oxytocin in parenting and as augmentative pharmacotherapy; critical issues and bold conjectures. J Neuroendocrinol. 2015. doi: 10.1111/jne.12355.
    1. Sannen J. De ontwikkeling en validering van het Middle Childhood Attachment Micro Observation system (MCAM) Belgium: KU Leuven; 2015.
    1. Roelandts L. Een micro-analytisch onderzoek naar interactiepatronen van moeders en lagere schoolkinderen. Belgium: KU Leuven; 2015.
    1. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:301–9.
    1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Riem MM, Tops M, Alink LR. Oxytocin decreases handgrip force in reaction to infant crying in females without harsh parenting experiences. Soc Cogn Affect Neur. 2011;7:951–7. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr067.
    1. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Hollander E, Ludwig NN, Kolevzon A, Ochsner KN. Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1426–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383439.
    1. Meinlschmidt G, Heim C. Sensitivity to intranasal oxytocin in adult men with early parental separation. Biol Psychiat. 2007;61:1109–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.007.
    1. Pahlke F, König IR, Ziegler A. Randomization In Treatment Arms (RITA): ein Randomisierungs-Programm für klinische Studien. Informatik Biometrie Epidemiol Med Biol. 2004;35:1–22.
    1. Doig GS, Simpson F. Randomization and allocation concealment: a practical guide for researchers. J Crit Care. 2005;20:187–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrc.2005.04.005.
    1. Chan A-W, Tetzlaff JM, Gøtzsche PC, Altman DG, Mann H, Berlin J, et al. SPIRIT 2013 Explanation and Elaboration: guidance for protocols of clinical trials. BMJ. 2013;346:e7586. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7586.
    1. Van IJzendoorn MH, Bhandari R, Van der Veen R, Grewen KM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Elevated salivary levels of oxytocin persist more than 7 h after intranasal administration. Front Neurosci. 2012;6:174.
    1. Cella M, Knibbe C, Danhof M, Della PO. What is the right dose for children? Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2010;70:597–603. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2009.03591.x.
    1. MacDonald E, Dadds MR, Brennan JL, Williams K, Levy F, Cauchi AJ. A review of safety, side-effects and subjective reactions to intranasal oxytocin in human research. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2011;36:1114–26. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.02.015.
    1. Guastella AJ, Hickie IB, McGuinness MM, Otis M, Woods EA, Disinger HM, et al. Recommendations for the standardisation of oxytocin nasal administration and guidelines for its reporting in human research. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2013;38:612–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.019.
    1. Vandevivere E, Braet C, Bosmans G. Under which conditions do early adolescents need maternal support? J Early Adolescence. 2015;35:162–9. doi: 10.1177/0272431614529364.
    1. Salemink E, Van den Hout M. Validation of the “recognition task” used in the training of interpretation biases. J Behav Ther Exp Psy. 2010;41:140–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.11.006.
    1. Ridenour TA, Greenberg MT, Cook ET. Structure and validity of people in my life: a self-report measure of attachment in late childhood. J Youth Adolescence. 2006;35:1037–53. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-9070-5.
    1. Bosmans G, Braet C, Heylen J, De Raedt R. Children’s attentional processing of mother and proximity seeking. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0124038. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124038.
    1. Dujardin A, Santens T, Braet C, De Raedt R, Vos P, Maes B, Bosmans G. Middle childhood support-seeking behavior during stress: links with self-reported attachment and future depressive symptoms. Child Dev. 2016;87:326–40. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12491.
    1. Biringen Z, Robinson JL, Emde RN. Manual for scoring the emotional availability scales. Fort Collins: Colorado State University; 1998.
    1. Dowdy E, Twyford J, Sharkey JD. Methods of assessing behavior: observations and rating scales. In: Saklofske DH, Reynolds CR, Schwean V, editors. The Oxford handbook of child psychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 623–50.
    1. Jaccard P. The distribution of the flora in the alpine zone. New Phytol. 1912;11:37–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.1912.tb05611.x.
    1. Ellis LK, Rothbart MK. Revision of the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire. Minneapolis: Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; 2001.
    1. Ellis LK. Individual differences and adolescent psychosocial development. University of Oregon; 2002.
    1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont; 1991.
    1. Verhulst FC, Van der Ende J, Koot HM. Handleiding voor de CBCL/4-18 [Manual for the CBCL/4–18] Rotterdam: Erasmus University; 1996.
    1. De Groot A, Koot HM, Verhulst FC. Cross-cultural generalizability of the Child Behavior Checklist cross-informant syndromes. Psychol Assessment. 1994;6:225–30. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.6.3.225.
    1. Van Leeuwen K, Vermulst A. Handleiding bij de Verkorte Schaal voor Ouderlijk Gedrag [Manual of the short version of the Parental Behavior Scale] Leuven: KU Leuven; 2010.
    1. Delhaye M, Beyers W, Klimstra TA, Linkowski P, Goossens L. The Leuven Adolescent Perceived Parenting Scale (LAPPS): reliability and validity with French-speaking adolescents in Belgium. Psychol Belg. 2012;52:289–305. doi: 10.5334/pb-52-4-289.
    1. Grolnick WS, Ryan RM, Deci EL. Inner resources for school achievement: motivational mediators of children's perceptions of their parents. J Educ Psychol. 1991;83:508–17. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.83.4.508.
    1. Barber BK. Parental psychological control: revisiting a neglected construct. Child Dev. 1996;67:3296–319. doi: 10.2307/1131780.
    1. Beyers W, Goossens L. Dynamics of perceived parenting and identity formation in late adolescence. J Adolescence. 2008;31:165–84. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.04.003.
    1. Kins E, Beyers W, Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M. Patterns of home leaving and subjective well-being in emerging adulthood: the role of motivational processes and parental autonomy support. Dev Psychol. 2009;45:1416–29. doi: 10.1037/a0015580.
    1. Janssens A, Goossens L, Van Den Noortgate W, Colpin H, Verschueren K, Van Leeuwen K. Parents’ and adolescents’ perspectives on parenting: evaluating conceptual structure, measurement invariance and criterion validity. Assessment. 2015;22:473–89. doi: 10.1177/1073191114550477.
    1. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J Youth Adolescence. 1988;17:117–33. doi: 10.1007/BF01537962.
    1. Carskadon MA, Acebo C. A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. J Adolescent Health. 1993;14:190–5. doi: 10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9.
    1. Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, Pollak SD. Pubertal development: correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Dev. 2009;80:327–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.x.
    1. Piccirillo JF, Merritt MG, Richards ML. Psychometric and clinimetric validity of the 20-item Sino-Nasal Outcome Test (Snot-20) Otolaryng Head Neck. 2002;126:41–7. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2002.121022.
    1. De Winter S. Middle childhood attachment from a cognitive perspective: the role of interpretation processing biases (Doctoral dissertation). 2017. Retrieved from . Retrieved May 23 2017
    1. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146.
    1. Uvnäs Moberg K, Prime DK. Oxytocin effects in mothers and infants during breastfeeding. Infant. 2013;9:201–6.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa