Development and validation of a new methodological platform to measure behavioral, cognitive, and physiological responses to food interventions in real time

M A Vargas-Alvarez, H Al-Sehaim, J M Brunstrom, G Castelnuovo, S Navas-Carretero, J A Martínez, E Almiron-Roig, M A Vargas-Alvarez, H Al-Sehaim, J M Brunstrom, G Castelnuovo, S Navas-Carretero, J A Martínez, E Almiron-Roig

Abstract

To fully understand the causes and mechanisms involved in overeating and obesity, measures of both cognitive and physiological determinants of eating behavior need to be integrated. Effectively synchronizing behavioral measures such as meal micro-structure (e.g., eating speed), cognitive processing of sensory stimuli, and metabolic parameters, can be complex. However, this step is central to understanding the impact of food interventions on body weight. In this paper, we provide an overview of the existing gaps in eating behavior research and describe the development and validation of a new methodological platform to address some of these issues. As part of a controlled trial, 76 men and women self-served and consumed food from a buffet, using a portion-control plate with visual stimuli for appropriate amounts of main food groups, or a conventional plate, on two different days, in a random order. In both sessions participants completed behavioral and cognitive tests using a novel methodological platform that measured gaze movement (as a proxy for visual attention), eating rate and bite size, memory for portion sizes, subjective appetite and portion-size perceptions. In a sub-sample of women, hormonal secretion in response to the meal was also measured. The novel platform showed a significant improvement in meal micro-structure measures from published data (13 vs. 33% failure rate) and high comparability between an automated gaze mapping protocol vs. manual coding for eye-tracking studies involving an eating test (ICC between methods 0.85; 90% CI 0.74, 0.92). This trial was registered at Clinical Trials.gov with Identifier NCT03610776.

Keywords: Automatic Gaze Mapping; Portion size memory; Portion-control plate; Satiety; Universal Eating Monitor.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article. The funders and sponsors had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

© 2022. The Author(s).

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Potential mechanisms involved in portion control based on the effects of visual cues in tableware and food packaging. Two mediator pathways are considered, one at the time of serving (green arrows and circles); and one at the time of eating (orange arrows and circles). Stimuli are indicated in rectangles, mechanisms in circles and behaviors in triangles (see text for literature sources)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Daily procedure for participants and components of the combined methodological platform. Plates used for this study are depicted on the bottom, right (for details, see text). Abbreviations: PCSE, portion-control self-efficacy scale; TFEQ, three-factor eating questionnaire; UEM, universal eating monitor; VAS, visual analogue scale questionnaire. Portion-control plate picture courtesy of Precise Portions LLC, Virginia, USA
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Universal Eating Monitor station at the University of Navarra. Left and upper right, room setting displaying the function for visual analogue scale (VAS) questionnaire on one of the screen monitors. Bottom right, lateral view outline of the anti-vibratory table hosting the high precision balance (Diagram courtesy of Borda Laboratorios, Madrid, Spain)
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Screenshot of the portion-size reconstruction software. The food photos were taken by a professional photographer using a digital camera with constant lightning and angle, and the same control dish. Portion sizes started at the equivalent of 1 tablespoon and followed by 20 kcal increments until the food filled about 80% of the plate (assumed to be the maximum volume physically fitting in the plate, based on the study protocol, which required selecting at least three meal components). The initial 1 tablespoon portion is based on 50% of the average small portion of cooked rice for Spanish consumers (Russolillo & Marques, 2008). The 20 kcal increments are based on previous research using full plates (Brunstrom, 2014). For carrots, due to their low energy density, 5 kcal increments were used instead
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Development of the video data coding protocol with Lightworks 14.0. Left, steps followed for the protocol development. Right, areas of interest (AOI) used for the initial gaze data analysis. Dwell time on mixed food areas (including more than one AOI) and time on food loaded onto the fork was also initially included
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Results from the memory reconstruction task for the first 20 subjects completing the study. Bars depict the comparison of eaten vs. recalled portion sizes after using each plate. Data are means ± SEM. a Calibrated plate. b Control plate
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Frequency distribution of the differences between target and actual extraction times for extraction times at 5, 10, 30, 60, and 90 min for 76 volunteers. Values on the X-axis are seconds
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
Correlation for 14 pairs of data between proportional fixation times (% time on each AOI relative to total AOI time) as registered by two independent raters using the manual coding protocol in Lightworks 14.0 across two video-recordings
Fig. 9
Fig. 9
Bland–Altman plot of AOI fixation times (s) collected with two methods (manual coding vs. automated gaze mapping) over the first 60 min upon starting the meal. Differences between methods are plotted against the mean of both methods for the vegetables, rice, and meatballs AOIs across a sample of ten recordings (five subjects using both plates each; n = 30 pairs of data). Dotted lines indicate the upper and lower limits of agreement, respectively. The two values with a difference > 10 in the Y-axis correspond to the rice AOI for a single subject (both plates). The value < – 6 in the Y-axis corresponds to the vegetables AOI for another subject (calibrated plate). Both subjects were female, with overweight

References

    1. Allison DB, Baskin ML. Handbook of assessment methods for eating behaviours and weight-related problems. Measures, theory, and research. 2. Sage Publications, Inc; 2009.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Palla L, Guest K, Ricchiuti C, Vint N, Jebb SA, Drewnowski A. Factors that determine energy compensation: a systematic review of preload studies. Nutrition Reviews. 2013;71(7):458–473. doi: 10.1111/nure.12048.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Tsiountsioura M, Lewis HB, Wu JH, Solis-Trapala I, Jebb SA. Large portion sizes increase bite size and eating rate in overweight women. Physiology and Behavior. 2015;139:297–302. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.041.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Domínguez A, Vaughan D, Solis-Trapala I, Jebb SA. Acceptability and potential effectiveness of commercial portion control tools amongst people with obesity. British Journal of Nutrition. 2016;116(11):1974–1983. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516004104.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Navas-Carretero S, Emery P, Martinez JA. Research into food portion size: methodological aspects and applications. Food & Function. 2017;9(2):715–739. doi: 10.1039/C7FO01430A.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Forde CG, Hollands GJ, Vargas MÁ, Brunstrom JM. A review of evidence supporting current strategies, challenges, and opportunities to reduce portion sizes. Nutrition Reviews. 2019;78(2):91–114. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuz047.
    1. Almiron-Roig E, Majumdar A, Vaughan D, Jebb SA. Exploring the experiences of people with obesity using portion control tools—A qualitative study. Nutrients. 2019;11(5):1095. doi: 10.3390/nu11051095.
    1. Bartko JJ. The intraclass correlation coefficient as a measure of reliability. Psychological Reports. 1966;19:3–11. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1966.19.1.3.
    1. Benjamins, J. S., Hooge, I. T. C., Benedict, C., Smeets, P. A. M., & van der Laan, L. N. (2021). The influence of acute partial sleep deprivation on liking, choosing and consuming high- and low-energy foods. Food Quality and Preference, 88. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104074
    1. Benton D. Portion size: what we know and what we need to know. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition. 2015;77(7):988–1004. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2012.679980.
    1. Best M, Barsalou LW, Papies EK. Appetite. Academic Press; 2018. Studying human eating behaviour in the laboratory: Theoretical considerations and practical suggestions; pp. 339–343.
    1. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. The Lancet. 1986;327(8476):307–310. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(86)90837-8.
    1. Bollen Z, Masson N, Salvaggio S, D’Hondt F, Maurage P. Craving is everything: An eye-tracking exploration of attentional bias in binge drinking. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2020;34(6):636–647. doi: 10.1177/0269881120913131.
    1. Brunstrom JM. The control of meal size in human subjects: a role for expected satiety, expected satiation and premeal planning. The Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2011;70(2):155–161. doi: 10.1017/S002966511000491X.
    1. Brunstrom JM. Mind over platter: pre-meal planning and the control of meal size in humans. International Journal of Obesity. 2014;38(Suppl 1):S9–12. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.83.
    1. Brunstrom JM, Rogers PJ. How many calories are on our plate? Expected fullness, not liking, determines meal-size selection. Obesity. 2009;17(10):1884–1890. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.201.
    1. Brunstrom JM, Rogers PJ, Pothos EM, Calitri R, Tapper K. Estimating everyday portion size using a ‘method of constant stimuli’: In a student sample, portion size is predicted by gender, dietary behaviour, and hunger, but not BMI. Appetite. 2008;51(2):296–301. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.005.
    1. Brunstrom JM, Collingwood J, Rogers PJ. Perceived volume, expected satiation, and the energy content of self-selected meals. Appetite. 2010;55(1):25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.005.
    1. Castellanos EH, Charboneau E, Dietrich MS, Park S, Bradley BP, Mogg K, Cowan RL. Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: Evidence for altered reward system function. International Journal of Obesity. 2009;33(9):1063–1073. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.138.
    1. Clement J. Visual influence on in-store buying decisions: an eye-track experiment on the visual influence of packaging design. Journal of Marketing Management. 2007;23(9–10):917–928. doi: 10.1362/026725707x250395.
    1. Crum AJ, Corbin WR, Brownell KD, Salovey P. Mind over milkshakes: mindsets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response. Health Psychology : Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2011;30(4):424–431. doi: 10.1037/a0023467.
    1. Deubel H. The time course of presaccadic attention shifts. Psychological Research. 2008;72(6):630–640. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0165-3.
    1. Duchowski AT. Eye Tracking Methodology. 3. Springer International Publishing; 2017. Eye Tracking Methodology. Theory and Practice.
    1. EASO. (2019). 12 Causes of Obesity - EASO. European Association for the Study of Obesity. . Accessed 07 Oct 2021
    1. English L, Lasschuijt M, Keller KL. Mechanisms of the portion size effect. What is known and where do we go from here? Appetite. 2015;88:39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.004.
    1. Fast LC, Harman JJ, Maertens JA, Burnette JL, Dreith F. Creating a measure of portion control self-efficacy. Eating Behaviors. 2015;16:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.009.
    1. Ford AL, Bergh C, Södersten P, Sabin MA, Hollinghurst S, Hunt LP, Shield JPH. Treatment of childhood obesity by retraining eating behaviour: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Ed.) 2009;340:b5388. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b5388.
    1. Forde CG, van Kuijk N, Thaler T, de Graaf C, Martin N. Texture and savoury taste influences on food intake in a realistic hot lunch time meal. Appetite. 2013;60(1):180–186. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.10.002.
    1. Forde C, Almiron-Roig E, Brunstrom JM. Expected Satiety: Application to weight management and understanding energy selection in humans. Current Obesity Reports. 2015;4(1):131–140. doi: 10.1007/s13679-015-0144-0.
    1. Fuchs AF. The saccadic system. In: Bach-y-Rita P, Collins CC, Hyde JE, editors. The control of eye movements. Academic Press; 1971. pp. 343–362.
    1. Garner DM, Bohr Y, Garfinkel PE. The Eating Attitudes Test: Psychometric Features and Clinical Correlates. Psychological Medicine. 1982;12(4):871–878. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700049163.
    1. Gough T, Gilchrist LD, Brunstrom JM. Investigating the role of familiarity, meal duration, and visual attention in the modelling of food intake. Appetite. 2018;123:468. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.080.
    1. Haynes A, Hardman CA, Makin ADJ, Halford JCG, Jebb SA, Robinson E. Visual perceptions of portion size normality and intended food consumption: A norm range model. Food Quality and Preference. 2019;72:77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.10.003.
    1. Haynes A, Hardman CA, Halford JCG, Jebb SA, Mead BR, Robinson E. Reductions to main meal portion sizes reduce daily energy intake regardless of perceived normality of portion size: A 5 day cross-over laboratory experiment. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2020;17(1):1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-0920-4.
    1. Herman PC, Polivy J, Pliner P, Vartanian LR. Mechanisms underlying the portion-size effect. Physiology & Behavior. 2015;144:129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.025.
    1. Hessels RS, Kemner C, van den Boomen C, Hooge ITC. The area-of-interest problem in eyetracking research: A noise-robust solution for face and sparse stimuli. Behavior Research Methods. 2016;48(4):1694–1712. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0676-y.
    1. Hetherington, M. M., & Rolls, B. J. (2018). Favouring more rigour when investigating human eating behaviour is like supporting motherhood and apple pie: A response to Robinson, Bevelander, Field, and Jones (2018). In: Appetite (Vol. 130, pp. 330–333). Academic Press. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.013
    1. Hill AJ, Blundell JE. Nutrients and behaviour: Research strategies for the investigation of taste characteristics, food preferences, hunger sensations and eating patterns in man. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1982;17(2):203–212. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90023-1.
    1. Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Lewis HB, Wei Y, Higgins JP, Ogilvie D. Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2015;9:CD011045. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011045.pub2.
    1. Holmqvist, K., & Andersson, R. (2017). Eyetracking: A comprehensive guide to methods, paradigms and measures (Issue March). . Accessed 05 Feb 2021
    1. Hopkins, W. G. (2009). Calculating the reliability intraclass correlation coefficient and its confidence limits (Excel spreadsheet). . . Accessed 28 Jun 2018
    1. Hvelplund, K. T. (2014). Eye tracking and the translation process: reflections on the analysis and interpretation of eye-tracking data. Minding Translation, MonTI Special Issue, 201–223. . Accessed 02 Feb 2021
    1. Irwin DE. Fixation Location and Fixation Duration as Indices of Cognitive Processing. In: John Henderson J, Ferreira F, editors. The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action. Eye Movements and the Visual World. 1. Routledge; 2004. pp. 105–134.
    1. Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical Significance: A Statistical Approach to Defining Meaningful Change in Psychotherapy Research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59(1):12–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12.
    1. Kissileff H. Ingestive behavior microstructure, basic mechanisms and clinical applications. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24(99):171–172. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00071-8.
    1. Kissileff HR, Klingsberg G, Van Ittalie TB. Universal eating monitor for continuous recording of solid or liquid consumption in man. The American Journal of Physiology. 1980;238:R14–R22.
    1. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine. 2016;15(2):155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012.
    1. Laessle RG, Lehrke S, Dückers S. Laboratory eating behavior in obesity. Appetite. 2007;49(2):399–404. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.11.010.
    1. Lasschuijt M, Mars M, De Graaf C, Smeets PAM. How oro-sensory exposure and eating rate affect satiation and associated endocrine responses - A randomized trial. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2020;111(6):1137–1149. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa067.
    1. Lewis HB, Forwood SE, Ahern AL, Verlaers K, Robinson E, Higgs S, Jebb SA. Personal and social norms for food portion sizes in lean and obese adults. International Journal of Obesity. 2015;39(8):1319–1324. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.47.
    1. Liu J, Tang W, Chen G, Lu Y, Feng C, Tu XM. Correlation and agreement: overview and clarification of competing concepts and measures. Shanghai Archives of. Psychiatry. 2016;28(2):115–120. doi: 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.216045.
    1. MacInnes, J. J., Iqbal, S., Pearson, J., & Johnson, E. N. (2018). Wearable Eye-tracking for Research: Automated dynamic gaze mapping and accuracy/precision comparisons across devices. In bioRxiv (p. 299925). bioRxiv. 10.1101/299925
    1. Marchiori D, Papies EK, Klein O. The portion size effect on food intake. An anchoring and adjustment process? Appetite. 2014;81:108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.018.
    1. McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods. 1996;1(1):30–46. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.30.
    1. McNally J, Hugh-Jones S, Caton S, Vereijken C, Weenen H, Hetherington MM. The eyes have it: Infant gaze as an indicator of hunger and satiation. Appetite. 2019;133:353–361. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.11.026.
    1. Mishra A, Mishra H, Masters TM. The influence of bite size on quantity of food consumed: a field study. Journal of Construction Research. 2011;38(5):791–795.
    1. Näslund E, Gutniak M, Skogar S, Rössner S, Hellström PM. Glucagon-like peptide 1 increases the period of postprandial satiety and slows gastric emptying in obese men. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1998;68(3):525–530. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/68.3.525.
    1. Orquin JL, Holmqvist K. Threats to the validity of eye-movement research in psychology. Behavior Research Methods. 2018;50(4):1645–1656. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0998-z.
    1. Orquin JL, Ashby NJS, Clarke ADF. Areas of Interest as a Signal Detection Problem in Behavioral Eye-Tracking Research. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2016;29(2–3):103–115. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1867.
    1. Petty AJ, Melanson KJ, Greene GW. Self-reported eating rate aligns with laboratory measured eating rate but not with free-living meals. Appetite. 2013;63:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.12.014.
    1. Rivas T, Bersabé R, Jiménez M, Berrocal C. The Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26): reliability and validity in Spanish female samples. The Spanish Journal of Psychology. 2010;13(2):1044–1056. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600002687.
    1. Robinson E, Kersbergen I. Portion size and later food intake: evidence on the “normalizing” effect of reducing food portion sizes. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2018;107(4):640–646. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy013.
    1. Robinson E, Aveyard P, Daley A, Jolly K, Lewis A, Lycett D, Higgs S. Eating attentively: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of food intake memory and awareness on eating. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013;97(4):728–742. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.045245.
    1. Robinson, E, Higgs, S., Daley, A. J., Jolly, K., Lycett, D., Lewis, A., & Aveyard, P. (2013b). Development and feasibility testing of a smart phone based attentive eating intervention. BMC Public Health, 13, 639. 10.1186/1471-2458-13-639
    1. Robinson E, Almiron-Roig E, Rutters F, de Graaf C, Forde CG, Tudur Smith C, Nolan SJ, Jebb SA. A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2014;100(1):123–151. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.081745.
    1. Robinson E, Kersbergen I, Brunstrom JM, Field M. I’m watching you. Awareness that food consumption is being monitored is a demand characteristic in eating-behaviour experiments. Appetite. 2014;83:19–25. doi: 10.1016/J.APPET.2014.07.029.
    1. Robinson E, Hardman CA, Halford JCG, Jones A. Eating under observation: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect that heightened awareness of observation has on laboratory measured energy intake. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2015;102(2):324–337. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.111195.
    1. Robinson E, Oldham M, Cuckson I, Brunstrom JM, Rogers PJ, Hardman CA. Visual exposure to large and small portion sizes and perceptions of portion size normality: Three experimental studies. Appetite. 2016;98:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.010.
    1. Rolls BJ. What is the role of portion control in weight management? International Journal of Obesity. 2014;38(Suppl 1):S1–8. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.82.
    1. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, James BL, Sanchez CE. Does the incorporation of portion-control strategies in a behavioral program improve weight loss in a 1-year randomized controlled trial? International Journal of Obesity. 2017;41(3):434–442. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2016.217.
    1. Russolillo G, Marques I. Food portion sizes photographic atlas [Álbum fotográfico de porciones de alimentos] Imagen y Comunicación Multimedia; 2008.
    1. Salvucci, D. D., & Anderson, J. A. (1998). Tracing Eye Movement Protocols with Cognitive Process Models. Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 923–928.
    1. Salvucci, D. D., & Goldberg, J. H. (2000). Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols. Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications - ETRA ’00, 71–78. 10.1145/355017.355028
    1. Sánchez-Carracedo D, Raich I Escursell RM, Figureas Piqueras M, Torras Clarasó J, Mora Giral M. Adaptación preliminar del cuestionario de alimentacion de Stunkard y Messick (Three Factor Eating Questionnaire, TFEQ) con una muestra Española universitaria. Psicología Conductual. 1999;7:393–416.
    1. Schwartz J, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Portion distortion: typical portion sizes selected by young adults. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2006;106:1412–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2006.06.006.
    1. Shimojo S, Simion C, Shimojo E, Scheier C. Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(12):1317–1322. doi: 10.1038/nn1150.
    1. Smeets PA, Erkner A, De Graaf C. Cephalic phase responses and appetite. Nutrition Reviews. 2010;68(11):643–655. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2010.00334.x.
    1. Steenhuis I, Poelman M. Portion Size: Latest Developments and Interventions. Current Obesity Reports. 2017;6(1):10–17. doi: 10.1007/s13679-017-0239-x.
    1. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1985;29:71–83. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8.
    1. Thibeault M, Jesteen M, Beitman A. Improved Accuracy Test Method for Mobile Eye Tracking in Usability Scenarios. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting. 2019;63(1):2226–2230. doi: 10.1177/1071181319631083.
    1. van der Laan LN, Papies EK, Hooge ITC, Smeets PAM. Goal-directed visual attention drives health goal priming: An eye-tracking experiment. Health Psychol Psychology. 2017;36(1):82–90. doi: 10.1037/hea0000410.
    1. Vargas MÁ, Brunstrom JM, Pérez S, Ciaurriz V, Sobrino S, Martínez de Morentin B, Navas-Carretero S, Martínez JA, Almiron-Roig E. Measuring the Effects of Visual Cues on Eating Behaviour through a Combined Methodological Platform: Pilot Study. In: Grant R, Allen T, Spink A, Sullivan M, editors. Measuring Behaviour 2018: 11th International Conference on Methods and Techniques in Behavioral Research Conference. Measuring Behavior 2010; 2018. pp. 82–90.
    1. Vargas MÁ, Martínez JA, Brunstrom JM, Navas-Carretero S, Almiron-Roig E. Impact of tableware visual cues on portion size, meal micro-structure and the satiety response in lean and overweight women. Obesity Facts. 2019;12(suppl1):130.
    1. Vargas-Alvarez MA, Navas-Carretero S, Palla L, Martínez JA, Almiron-Roig E. Impact of portion control tools on portion size awareness, choice and intake: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2021;13(6):1–38. doi: 10.3390/nu13061978.
    1. Vermeer WM, Steenhuis IHM, Poelman MP. Small, medium, large or supersize the development and evaluation of interventions targeted at portion size. International Journal of Obesity. 2014;38(SUPPL. 1):S13–8. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.84.
    1. Wadhera, D., & Capaldi-Phillips, E. D. (2014). A review of visual cues associated with food on food acceptance and consumption. In Eating Behaviors, 15(1). 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.11.003
    1. Wang E, Cakmak YO, Peng M. Eating with eyes – Comparing eye movements and food choices between overweight and lean individuals in a real-life buffet setting. Appetite. 2018;125:152–159. doi: 10.1016/J.APPET.2018.02.003.
    1. Wansink B. From mindless eating to mindlessly eating better. Physiology & Behavior. 2010;100(5):454–463. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.05.003.
    1. Wansink B, van Ittersum K. Bottoms Up! The Influence of Elongation on Pouring and Consumption Volume. Journal of Consumer Research. 2003;30(3):455–463. doi: 10.1086/378621.
    1. Werthmann J, Roefs A, Nederkoorn C, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Jansen A. Can(not) Take my Eyes off it: Attention Bias for Food in Overweight Participants. Health Psychology. 2011;30(5):561–569. doi: 10.1037/a0024291.
    1. Westerterp-Plantenga, M. S. (2000). Eating behavior in humans, characterized by cumulative food intake curves--a review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(2), 239–248.
    1. Wilkinson LL, Hinton EC, Fay SH, Ferriday D, Rogers PJ, Brunstrom JM. Computer-based assessments of expected satiety predict behavioural measures of portion-size selection and food intake. Appetite. 2012;59(3):933–938. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.007.
    1. Wilkinson LL, Ferriday D, Bosworth ML, Godinot N, Martin N, Rogers PJ, Brunstrom JM. Keeping Pace with Your Eating: Visual Feedback Affects Eating Rate in Humans. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):e0147603. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147603.
    1. Woodward, E., Haszard, J., Worsfold, A., & Venn, B. (2020). Comparison of self-reported speed of eating with an objective measure of eating rate. Nutrients, 12(3). 10.3390/nu12030599
    1. World Health Organization . Obesity and Overweight. World Health Organization; 2020.
    1. Yeomans MR. Rating changes over the course of meals: what do they tell us about motivation to eat? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24(2):249–259. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00078-0.
    1. Yeomans, M. R. (2018). Measuring Appetite and Food Intake. In: Methods in Consumer Research (vol 2, pp. 119–149). 10.1016/B978-0-08-101743-2.00006-6
    1. Yeomans MR, Re R, Wickham MS, Lundholm H, Chambers L. Beyond expectations: the physiological basis of sensory enhancement of satiety. International Journal of Obesity. 2016;40(11):1693–1698. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2016.112.
    1. Zlatevska N, Dubelaar C, Holden SS. Sizing Up the Effect of Portion Size on Consumption: A Meta-Analytic Review. Journal of Marketing. 2014;78(3):140–154. doi: 10.1509/jm.12.0303.

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi