Graph analysis of verbal fluency test discriminate between patients with Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and normal elderly controls

Laiss Bertola, Natália B Mota, Mauro Copelli, Thiago Rivero, Breno Satler Diniz, Marco A Romano-Silva, Sidarta Ribeiro, Leandro F Malloy-Diniz, Laiss Bertola, Natália B Mota, Mauro Copelli, Thiago Rivero, Breno Satler Diniz, Marco A Romano-Silva, Sidarta Ribeiro, Leandro F Malloy-Diniz

Abstract

Verbal fluency is the ability to produce a satisfying sequence of spoken words during a given time interval. The core of verbal fluency lies in the capacity to manage the executive aspects of language. The standard scores of the semantic verbal fluency test are broadly used in the neuropsychological assessment of the elderly, and different analytical methods are likely to extract even more information from the data generated in this test. Graph theory, a mathematical approach to analyze relations between items, represents a promising tool to understand a variety of neuropsychological states. This study reports a graph analysis of data generated by the semantic verbal fluency test by cognitively healthy elderly (NC), patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment-subtypes amnestic (aMCI) and amnestic multiple domain (a+mdMCI)-and patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). Sequences of words were represented as a speech graph in which every word corresponded to a node and temporal links between words were represented by directed edges. To characterize the structure of the data we calculated 13 speech graph attributes (SGA). The individuals were compared when divided in three (NC-MCI-AD) and four (NC-aMCI-a+mdMCI-AD) groups. When the three groups were compared, significant differences were found in the standard measure of correct words produced, and three SGA: diameter, average shortest path, and network density. SGA sorted the elderly groups with good specificity and sensitivity. When the four groups were compared, the groups differed significantly in network density, except between the two MCI subtypes and NC and aMCI. The diameter of the network and the average shortest path were significantly different between the NC and AD, and between aMCI and AD. SGA sorted the elderly in their groups with good specificity and sensitivity, performing better than the standard score of the task. These findings provide support for a new methodological frame to assess the strength of semantic memory through the verbal fluency task, with potential to amplify the predictive power of this test. Graph analysis is likely to become clinically relevant in neurology and psychiatry, and may be particularly useful for the differential diagnosis of the elderly.

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease; elderly; graph analysis; mild cognitive impairment; semantic verbal fluency.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(A) Representation of the word sequence produced on the Semantic Verbal Fluency task. (B) Representations of networks generated by NC, MCI, and AD subjects during the Semantic Verbal Fluency task.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Speech Graph Attributes (SGA) differentiates psychopathological groups. (A) SGA boxplots with significant differences among Alzheimer Disorder (AD), Moderate Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and control groups (N = 25 on AD and C group, N = 50 on MCI group; Kruskal-Wallis test followed by two-sided Wilcoxon Rank-sum test with Bonferroni correction with alpha = 0.0167). (B) Percentage of subjects in each group that made one L3 on the verbal fluency test. AD subjects showed more L3 than MCI subjects (Wilcoxon Rank-sum test with Bonferroni correction with alpha = 0.0167, p = 0.0090). (C) Rating quality measured by AUC, sensitivity and specificity, using MMSE or SGA correlated with clinical symptoms measured with MMSE and Lawton scales (Table 3) (attributes: WC, N, E, Density, Diameter, and ASP). Notice that SGA was more specific than MMSE on triple group sorting, and on MCI diagnosis against the control group. *p = 0.0167.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Speech Graph Attributes (SGA) differentiates psychopathological MCI subgroups. (A) SGA boxplots with significant differences among Alzheimer Disorder (AD), Amnesic Moderate Cognitive Impairment (aMCI), Multiple Domain Moderate Cognitive Impairment (a+mdMCI), and control groups indicated (N = 25 per group; Kruskal-Wallis test followed by two-sided Wilcoxon Rank-sum test with Bonferroni correction with alpha = 0.0083). (B) Rating quality measured by AUC, sensitivity and specificity, using SGA correlated with clinical symptoms measured with MMSE and Lawton scales (Table 4) (attributes: WC, N, E, Density, Diameter, and ASP). Notice that it is possible to sort the MCI subgroups from the NC or AD groups, but not one from another. Classification quality was considered excellent when AUC was higher than 0.8, good when AUC ranged from 0.6 to 0.8, and poor (not above the chance), when AUC was smaller than 0.6. *p = 0.0083.

References

    1. Adlam A.-L. R., Bozeat S., Arnold R., Watson P., Hodges J. R. (2006). Semantic knowledge in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 42, 675–684 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70404-0
    1. Amieva H., Jacqmin-Gadda H., Orgogozo J.-M., Le Carret N., Helmer C., Letenneur L., et al. (2005). The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study. Brain 128, 1093–1101 10.1093/brain/awh451
    1. Aretouli E., Okonkwo C. O., Samek J., Brandt J. (2011). The fate of the 0.5s: predictors of 2-year outcome in mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 277–288 10.1017/s1355617710001621
    1. Bales M. E., Johnson S. B. (2006). Graph theoretic modeling of large-scale semantic networks. J. Biomed. Inform. 39, 451–464 10.1016/j.jbi.2005.10.007
    1. Brucki S. M. D., Malheiros S. M. F., Okamoto I. H., Bertolucci P. H. F. (1997). Dados normativos para o teste de fluência verbal categoria animais em nosso meio. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 55, 56–61
    1. Butts C. T. (2009). Revisiting the foundations of network analysis. Science 325, 414–416 10.1126/science.1171022
    1. Craik F. I. M., Bialystok E. (2006). Cognition through the lifespan: mechanisms of change. Trends Cog. Sci. 10, 131–138 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.007
    1. De Deyne S., Storms G. (2008). Word associations: networ and semantic properties. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 213–231
    1. de Paula J. J., Bertola L., Ávila R. T., Moreira L., Coutinho G., Moraes E. N., et al. (2013). Clinical applicability and cutoff values for an unstructured neuropsychological assessment protocol for older adults with low formal education. PLoS ONE 8:E73167 10.1371/journal.pone.007316
    1. Diniz B. S. O., Yassuda M. S., Nunes P. V., Radanovic M., Forlenza O. V. (2007). Mini-mental state examination performance in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Int. Psychogeriatr. 19, 647–656 10.1590/S1516-44462008000400003
    1. Garrard P., Lambon Ralph M. A., Patterson K., Pratt K. H., Hodges J. R. (2005). Semantic feature knowledge and picture naming in dementia of Alzheimer's type: a new approach. Brain Lang. 93, 79–94 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.003
    1. Goni J., Arrondo G., Sepulcre J., Martincorena I., Mendizabal N. V., Corominas-Murtra B., et al. (2010). The semantic organization of the animal category: evidence from semantic verbal fluency and network theory. Cogn. Process. 12, 183–196 10.1007/s10339-010-0372-x
    1. Griffiths T. L., Steyvers M., Tenenbaum J. B. (2007). Topics in semantic representation. Psychol. Rev. 114, 211–244 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
    1. Henry J. D., Crawford J. R., Phillips L. H. (2004). Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer's type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 42, 1212–1222 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
    1. Hodges J. R., Erzinçlioglu S., Patterson K. (2006). Evolution of cognitive defi cits and conversion to dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a very-long-term follow-up study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21, 380–391 10.1159/000092534
    1. Holler Y., Trinka E. (2014). What do temporal lobe epilepsy and progressive mild cognitive impairment have in common? Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:58 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00058
    1. Huntley J. D., Howard R. J. (2010). Working memory in early Alzheimer's disease: a neuropsychological review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 121–132 10.1002/gps.2314
    1. Kotsiantis S. B. (2007). Supervised machine learning: a review of classification techniques. in Emerging Artificial Intelligence Applications in Computer Engineering: Real Word Ai Systems with Applications (Amsterdam: IOS Press; ), 3–24
    1. Lerner A. J., Ogrocki P. K., Thomas P. T. (2009). Network graph analysis of category fluency testing. Cog. Behav. Neurol. 22, 45–52 10.1097/wnn.0b013e318192ccaf
    1. Lezak M., Howieson D. B., Loring D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press
    1. McClelland J. L., Rogers T. T. (2003). The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 310–323 10.1038/nrn1076
    1. McKhann G., Drachman D., Folstein M., Katzman R., Price D., Stadlan E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer's. Neurology 34, 939–944 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939
    1. Mota M. B., Furtado R., Maia P. P. C., Copelli M., Ribeiro S. (2014). Graph analysis of dream reports is especially informative about psychosis. Sci. Rep. 4:3691 10.1038/srep03691
    1. Mota N. B., Vasconcelos N. A. P., Lemos N., Pieretti A. C., Kinouchi O., Cecchi G. A., et al. (2012). Speech graphs provide a quantitative measure of thought disorder in psychosis. PLoS ONE 7:e34928 10.1371/journal.pone.0034928
    1. Nickles L. (2001). Spoken word production, in What Deficits Reveal about the Human Mind/Brain: A Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology, ed Rapp B. (Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press; ), 291–320
    1. Nutter-Upham K. E., Saykin A. J., Rabin L. A., Roth R. M., Wishart H. A., Pare N., et al. (2008). Verbal fluency performance in amnestic MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. Achiev. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 229–241 10.1016/j.acn.2008.01.005
    1. Patterson K., Nestor P. J., Rogers T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 976–988 10.1038/nrn2277
    1. Radanovic M., Diniz B. S., Mirandez R. M., Novaretti T. M. S., Flacks M. K., Yassuda M. S., et al. (2009). Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease among Brazilian portuguese speakers: the influence of education. Int. Psychogeriatr. 21, 1–7 10.1017/S1041610209990639
    1. Salmon D. P., Thomas R. G., Pay M. M., Booth A., Hofstetter C. R., Thal L. J., et al. (2002). Alzheimer's disease can be accurately diagnosed in very mildly impaired individuals. Neurology 59, 1022–1028 10.1212/wnl.59.7.1022
    1. Saxton J., Lopez O. L., Ratcliff G., Dulberg C., Fried L. P., Carlson M. C., et al. (2004). Preclinical Alzheimer disease: neuropsychological test performance 1.5 to 8 years prior to onset. Neurology 63, 2341–2347 10.1017/S1041610208007631
    1. Singh M., Provan G. M. (1995). A comparison of induction algorithms for selective and non-selective Bayesian classifiers. Mach. Learn. Proc. 1995, 497–505
    1. Strauss E., Sherman E. M. S., Spreen O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
    1. Taler V., Phillips N. A. (2008). Language performance in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment: a comparative review. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 30, 501–556 10.1080/13803390701550128
    1. Unsworth N., Spillers G. J., Brewer G. A. (2011). Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 64, 447–466 10.1080/17470218.2010.505292
    1. Winblad B., Palmer K., Kivipelto M., Jelic V., Fratiglioni L., Wahlund L. O., et al. (2004). Mild Cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, towards a consensus: reports of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Med. 256, 240–246 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi