Remote Microphone Hearing Aid Use Improves Classroom Listening, Without Adverse Effects on Spatial Listening and Attention Skills, in Children With Auditory Processing Disorder: A Randomised Controlled Trial

Georgios Stavrinos, Vasiliki Vivian Iliadou, Menelaos Pavlou, Doris-Eva Bamiou, Georgios Stavrinos, Vasiliki Vivian Iliadou, Menelaos Pavlou, Doris-Eva Bamiou

Abstract

Background: Children with Auditory Processing Disorder (APD) often have poor auditory processing skills in the presence of normal peripheral hearing. These children have worse listening-in-noise skills compared to typically developing peers, while other commonly reported symptoms include poor attention and distractibility. One of the management strategies for children with APD is the use of Remote Microphone Hearing Aids (RMHAs), which can help improve the signal-to-noise ratio in the child's ears. The aim of this randomised controlled trial was to examine whether RMHAs improved classroom listening in children with APD, and to further test their effects on children's listening-in-noise and attention skills following a 6-month intervention.

Methods: Twenty-six children diagnosed with APD, aged 7-12, in primary mainstream education, were randomised into the intervention (N = 13) and control group (N = 13). The primary outcome measure was the Listening Inventory for Education - Revised questionnaire, completed by children to assess their listening using RMHAs under several acoustically challenging situations in the classroom. Secondary outcome measures included the Listening in Spatialised Noise - Sentences test, assessing speech-in-noise perception and spatial listening, and the Test of Everyday Attention for Children, assessing different types of attention skills. Tests were conducted in unaided conditions. Mixed analysis of variance was used to analyse the data. The clinical trial was registered at clinicaltrials.gov (unique identifier: NCT02353091).

Results: The questionnaire scores of self-reported listening skills in the classroom significantly improved in the intervention group after 3, MD = 7.31, SE = 2.113, p = 0.014, and after 6 months, M = 5.00, SE = 1.468, p = 0.016. The behavioural measures of listening-in-noise and attention did not significantly change.

Conclusion: Use of RMHAs improves classroom listening, evidenced by the results of the questionnaire analysis, while a 6-month use did not have adverse effects on unaided spatial listening or attention skills.

Keywords: attention; audiology; auditory processing disorder; randomised controlled trial; remote microphone hearing aids; spatial listening.

Copyright © 2020 Stavrinos, Iliadou, Pavlou and Bamiou.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Flow diagram showing the attrition through the different stages of the clinical trial. Three children were excluded after enrolment as they did not meet the inclusion criteria (see relevant section), while there was no loss of participants due to follow-up. APD, Auditory Processing Disorder; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Plots of mean scores in the LIFE-R Total Score, for 12 controls and 13 RMHA children, at baseline, 3 and at 6 months. Error bars represent a 95% CI of the mean. The scores of the intervention group significantly improved from baseline to 3 months, as well as from baseline to 6 months. This was a non-standardised questionnaire, therefore there was no indication of a cut-off for abnormal performance. CI, Confidence Interval; LIFE-R, Listening Inventory for Education – Revised; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid. ∗p < 0.05.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Plots of mean z scores in (A) the Low-cue SRT score, and (B) the Talker Advantage score, for 13 controls and 13 RMHA children, at baseline, 3 and 6 months. Error bars represent a 95% CI of the mean. The horizontal dotted line indicates the cut-off for what is considered abnormal performance (i.e., −2 z scores and below). CI, Confidence Interval; LiSN-S, Listening in Spatialised Noise – Sentences; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid; SiN, Speech-in-Noise; SRT, Speech Reception Threshold.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Plots of mean z scores in (A) the High-cue SRT score, (B) the Spatial Advantage score, and (C) the Total Advantage score, for 13 controls and 12 RMHA children (and 13 controls and 13 RMHA children in the Spatial Advantage score only), at baseline, 3 months and at 6 months. Error bars represent a 95% CI of the mean. The horizontal dotted line indicates the cut-off for what is considered abnormal performance (i.e., −2 z scores and below). One participant had two outlying cases in post-intervention testing at 6 months in the High-cue SRT and Total Advantage scores. Outliers were removed, as the participant had decreased focus and interest and had difficulty remaining seated during the fourth and final test condition. This condition is used to calculate both the High-cue SRT and Total Advantage scores, and because it is the last condition it can indicate auditory fatigue or declining attention in the subject (National Acoustic Laboratories, 2010). Removal of this case resulted in a comparison of 13 controls against 12 RMHA children in the analyses of these two conditions. CI, Confidence Interval; LiSN-S, Listening in Spatialised Noise – Sentences; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid; SRT, Speech Reception Threshold.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Plots of mean scaled scores in (A) the TEACh Sus-AA subtest, and (B) the TEACh Div-AVA subtest, for 13 controls and 13 RMHA children, at baseline, 3 and 6 months. Error bars represent a 95% CI of the mean. There was a significant difference between the two groups in the Div-AVA baseline scores, and significant difference in the Div-AVA scores of the intervention group from baseline to 6 months. The horizontal dotted line indicates the cut-off for what is considered abnormal performance (i.e., 4 scaled scores and below). CI, Confidence Interval; Div-AVA, Divided Auditory-Visual Attention; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid; Sus-AA, Sustained Auditory Attention; TEACh, Test of Everyday Attention for Children. ∗∗p < 0.01.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Plots of mean scaled scores in (A) the TEACh Sel-VA subtest, and (B) the TEACh Div-AA subtest, for 13 controls and 13 RMHA children, at baseline, 3 and 6 months. Error bars represent a 95% CI of the mean. The horizontal dotted line indicates the cut-off for what is considered abnormal performance (i.e., 4 scaled scores and below). CI, Confidence Interval; Div-AA, Divided Auditory Attention; RMHA, Remote Microphone Hearing Aid; Sel-VA, Selective Visual Attention; TEACh, Test of Everyday Attention for Children.

References

    1. Allen P., Allan C. (2014). Auditory processing disorders: relationship to cognitive processes and underlying auditory neural integrity. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinol. 78 198–208. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2013.10.048
    1. American Academy of Audiology (2010). American Academy of Audiology Clinical Practice Guidelines. Diagnosis, Treatment and Management of Children and Adults with Central Auditory Processing Disorder. Reston, VA: American Academy of Audiology.
    1. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (2005). (Central) Auditory Processing Disorders. Technical Report. Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
    1. Anderson K. L., Smaldino J. J., Spangler C. (2011). LIFE-R The Listening Inventories for Education-Revised. Success for Kids with Hearing Loss. Available online at: (accessed August 11, 2020).
    1. Anderson S., White-Schwoch T., Parbery-Clark A., Kraus N. (2013). A dynamic auditory-cognitive system supports speech-in-noise perception in older adults. Hear. Res. 300 18–32. 10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.006
    1. Bland J. M., Altman D. G. (1994). Some examples of regression towards the mean. BMJ 309:780. 10.1136/bmj.309.6957.780
    1. Box G. E. P. (1953). Non-normality and tests on variances. Biometrika 40:318 10.2307/2333350
    1. British Society of Audiology (2011). Practice Guidance. An Overview of Current Management of Auditory Processing Disorder (APD). Seafield: British Society of Audiology.
    1. British Society of Audiology (2012). Recommended Procedure. Pure-Tone Air-Conduction and Bone-Conduction Threshold Audiometry With and Without Masking. Seafield: British Society of Audiology; (Issue February).
    1. British Society of Audiology (2013). Recommended Procedure. Tympanometry. Seafield: British Society of Audiology; (Issue August).
    1. British Society of Audiology (2018). Position Statement and Practice Guidance Auditory Processing Disorder (APD). Seafield: British Society of Audiology.
    1. Cameron S., Dillon H. (2007). Development of the listening in spatialized noise-sentences test (LiSN-S). Ear Hear. 28 196–211. 10.1080/14992020601164170
    1. Cameron S., Dillon H. (2008). The listening in spatialized noise–sentences test (LISN-S): comparison to the prototype LISN and results from children with either a suspected (Central) auditory processing disorder or a confirmed language disorder. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 19 377–391. 10.3766/jaaa.19.5.2
    1. Cameron S., Dillon H. (2011). Development and evaluation of the LiSN & learn auditory training software for deficit-specific remediation of binaural processing deficits in children: preliminary findings. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 22 678–696. 10.3766/jaaa.22.10.6
    1. Cameron S., Dillon H., Newall P. (2006). The listening in spatialized noise test: an auditory processing disorder study. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 320 306–320. 10.3766/jaaa.17.5.2
    1. Cameron S., Glyde H., Dillon H. (2012). Efficacy of the LiSN & learn auditory training software: randomized blinded controlled study. Audiol. Res. 2 86–93. 10.4081/audiores.2012.e15
    1. Coch D., Sanders L. D., Neville H. J. (2005). An event-related potential study of selective auditory attention in children and adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17 605–622. 10.1162/0898929053467631
    1. Cohen J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    1. Dawes P., Munro K. J., Kalluri S., Edwards B. (2014). Acclimatization to hearing aids. Ear Hear. 35 203–212. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182a8eda4
    1. Dillon H., Cameron S., Glyde H., Wilson W. J., Tomlin D. (2012). An Opinion on the assessment of people who may have an auditory processing disorder. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 23 97–105. 10.3766/jaaa.23.2.4
    1. Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., Buchner A. (2007). G∗Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 175–191. 10.3758/BF03193146
    1. Fey M. E., Richard G. J., Geffner D., Kamhi A. G., Medwetsky L., Paul D., et al. (2011). Auditory processing disorder and auditory/language interventions: an evidence-based systematic review. Lang. Speech Hear. Services Schools 42 246–264. 10.1044/0161-1461(2010/10-0013)APD
    1. Filippini R., Befi-Lopes D. M., Schochat E. (2012). Efficacy of auditory training using the auditory brainstem response to complex sounds: auditory processing disorder and specific language impairment. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica 64 217–226. 10.1159/000342139
    1. Great Ormond Street Hospital (2018). Auditory Processing Disorder (APD). Clinic at Great Ormond Street Hospital. Referral Criteria & Further Information. London: Great Ormond Street Hospital.
    1. Gupta S. K. (2011). Intention-to-treat concept: a review. Perspect. Clin. Res. 2 109–112. 10.4103/2229-3485.83221
    1. Head J., Helton W. S. (2015). Passive perceptual learning versus active searching in a novel stimuli vigilance task. Exp. Brain Res. 233 1481–1489. 10.1007/s00221-015-4222-z
    1. Hickey G. L., Grant S. W., Dunning J., Siepe M. (2018). Statistical primer: sample size and power calculations-why, when and how? Eur. J. Cardio Thor. Surg. 54 4–9. 10.1093/ejcts/ezy169
    1. Hornickel J., Zecker S. G., Bradlow A. R., Kraus N. (2012). Assistive listening devices drive neuroplasticity in children with dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 16731–16736. 10.1073/pnas.1206628109
    1. Iliadou V. V., Ptok M., Grech H., Pedersen E. R., Brechmann A., Deggouj N., et al. (2017). A European perspective on auditory processing disorder – current knowledge and future research focus. Front. Neurol. 8:622. 10.3389/fneur.2017.00622
    1. Johnston K. N., John A. B., Kreisman N. V., Hall J. W., III, Crandell C. C. (2009). Multiple benefits of personal FM system use by children with auditory processing disorder (APD). Int. J. Audiol. 48 371–383. 10.1080/14992020802687516
    1. Kang M., Ragan B. G., Park J.-H. (2008). Issues in outcomes research: an overview of randomization techniques for clinical trials. J. Athletic Train. 43 215–221. 10.4085/1062-6050-43.2.215
    1. Keith R. W. (2000). Random Gap Detection Test. St. Louis, MO: Auditec.
    1. Keith R. W. (2009). SCAN-3 for Children. Tests for Auditory Processing Disorders. Manual, 2nd Edn London: Pearson, Inc.
    1. Keith W. J., Purdy S. C. (2014). Assistive and therapeutic effects of amplification for auditory processing disorder. Semin. Hear. 35 27–38. 10.1055/s-0033-1363522
    1. Kitterick P. T., Lovett R. E. S., Goman A. M., Summerfield A. Q. (2011). The AB-York crescent of sound: an apparatus for assessing spatial-listening skills in children and adults. Cochlear Implants Int. 12 164–169. 10.1179/146701011X13049348987832
    1. Koohi N., Vickers D. A., Warren J., Werring D., Bamiou D.-E. (2017). Long-term use benefits of personal frequency-modulated systems for speech in noise perception in patients with stroke with auditory processing deficits: a non-randomised controlled trial study. BMJ Open 7:e013003. 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013003
    1. Kuk F., Jackson A., Keenan D., Lau C.-C. (2008). Personal amplification for school-age children with auditory processing disorders. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 19 465–480. 10.3766/jaaa.19.6.3
    1. Lagacé J., Jutras B., Giguère C., Gagné J.-P. (2011). Speech perception in noise: exploring the effect of linguistic context in children with and without auditory processing disorder. Int. J. Audiol. 50 385–395. 10.3109/14992027.2011.553204
    1. Loo J. H. Y., Rosen S., Bamiou D.-E. (2016). Auditory training effects on the listening skills of children with auditory processing disorder. Ear Hear. 37 38–47. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000225
    1. Manly T., Robertson I. H., Anderson V., Nimmo-Smith I. (1999). TEA-Ch. The Test of Everyday Attention for Children. Karnataka: Pearson Assessment.
    1. Mesgarani N., Chang E. F. (2012). Selective cortical representation of attended speaker in multi-talker speech perception. Nature 485 233–236. 10.1038/nature11020
    1. Misra S. (2012). Randomized double blind placebo control studies, the “gold standard” in intervention based studies. Indian J. Sexually Transmitted Dis. 33 131–134. 10.4103/0253-7184.102130
    1. Murphy C. F. B., Hashim E., Dillon H., Bamiou D.-E. (2019). British Children’s performance on the listening in spatialised noise-sentences test (LISN-S). Int. J. Audiol. 58 1–7. 10.1080/14992027.2019.1627592
    1. Musiek F. E. (1994). Frequency (Pitch) and duration pattern tests. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 5 265–268.
    1. Musiek F. E., Gollegly K. M., Kibbe K. S., Verkest-Lenz S. B. (1991). Proposed screening test for central auditory disorders: follow-up on the dichotic digits test. Am. J. Otol. 12 109–113.
    1. Musiek F. E., Shinn J. B., Jirsa R., Bamiou D.-E., Baran J. A., Zaida E. (2005). GIN (Gaps-In-Noise) test performance in subjects with confirmed central auditory nervous system involvement. Ear Hear. 26 608–618. 10.1097/01.aud.0000188069.80699.41
    1. National Acoustic Laboratories (2010). Interpreting LiSN-S Results and Recommendations for Management. Sydney: Macquarie University, 1–4.
    1. Nordstokke D. W., Zumbo B. D. (2007). A cautionary tale about Levene’s tests for equal variances. J. Educ. Res. Policy Stud. 7 1–14.
    1. O’Sullivan J. A., Power A. J., Mesgarani N., Rajaram S., Foxe J. J., Shinn-Cunningham B. G., et al. (2015). Attentional selection in a cocktail party environment can be decoded from single-trial EEG. Cereb. Cortex 25 1697–1706. 10.1093/cercor/bht355
    1. Rance G., Corben L. A., Du Bourg E., King A., Delatycki M. B. (2010). Successful treatment of auditory perceptual disorder in individuals with friedreich ataxia. Neuroscience 171 552–555. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.09.013
    1. Richardson J. T. E. (2011). ETA squared and partial ETA squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ. Res. Rev. 6 135–147. 10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001
    1. Rocha-Muniz C. N., Zachi E. C., Teixeira R. A. A., Ventura D. F., Befi-Lopes D. M., Schochat E. (2014). Association between language development and auditory processing disorders. Br. J. Otorhinol. 80 231–236. 10.1016/j.bjorl.2014.01.002
    1. Sharma M., Dhamani I., Leung J., Carlile S. (2014). Attention, memory, and auditory processing in 10- to 15-year-old children with listening difficulties. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 57 2308–2321. 10.1044/2014
    1. Sharma M., Purdy S. C., Kelly A. S. (2012). A randomized control trial of interventions in school-aged children with auditory processing disorders. Int. J. Audiol. 51 506–518. 10.3109/14992027.2012.670272
    1. Shield B., Conetta R., Dockrell J., Connolly D., Cox T., Mydlarz C. (2015). A survey of acoustic conditions and noise levels in secondary school classrooms in England. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 137 177–188. 10.1121/1.4904528
    1. Shinn J. B., Chermak G. D., Musiek F. E. (2009). GIN (Gaps-In-Noise) performance in the pediatric population. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 20 229–238. 10.3766/jaaa.20.4.3
    1. Smart J. L., Purdy S. C., Kelly A. S. (2018). Impact of personal frequency modulation systems on behavioral and cortical auditory evoked potential measures of auditory processing and classroom listening in school-aged children with auditory processing disorder. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 19 1–19. 10.3766/jaaa.16074
    1. Snowling M. J., Hulme C. (2003). A critique of claims from reynolds, Nicolson & Hambly (2003) that DDAT is an effective treatment for children with reading difficulties – “lies, damned lies and (inappropriate) statistics?”. Dyslexia 9 127–133. 10.1002/dys.245
    1. Stavrinos G. (2019). Effects of Remote Microphone Hearing Aids (RMHAs) on Listening-in-Noise, Attention and Memory in School-Aged Children with Auditory Processing Disorder (APD). London: University College London.
    1. Stavrinos G., Iliadou V. V., Edwards L., Sirimanna T., Bamiou D.-E. (2018). The relationship between types of attention and auditory processing skills: reconsidering auditory processing disorder diagnosis. Front. Psychol. 9:34. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00034
    1. Tomlin D., Dillon H., Sharma M., Rance G. (2015). The impact of auditory processing and cognitive abilities in children. Ear Hear. 36 527–542. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000172
    1. Tomlin D., Rance G. (2014). Long-term hearing deficits after childhood middle ear disease. Ear Hear. 35 e233–e242. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000065
    1. Umat C., Mukari S. Z., Ezan N. F., Din N. C. (2011). Changes in auditory memory performance following the use of frequency-modulated system in children with suspected auditory processing disorders. Saudi Med. J. 32 818–824.
    1. Wechsler D., Naglieri J. A. (2006). Wechsler Nonverbal Scale of Ability. Technical and Interpretive Manual. London: Pearson, Inc.
    1. Wolraich M., Brown L., Brown R. T., DuPaul G., Earls M., Feldman H. M., et al. (2011). ADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 128 1007–1022. 10.1542/peds.2011-2654
    1. Yathiraj A., Vanaja C. S. (2015). Age related changes in auditory processes in children aged 6 to 10 years. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinol. 79 1224–1234. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.05.018
    1. Zion Golumbic E. M., Ding N., Bickel S., Lakatos P., Schevon C. A., McKhann G. M., et al. (2013). Mechanisms underlying selective neuronal tracking of attended speech at a “cocktail party.” Neuron 77 980–991. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.037

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi