Human hippocampal replay during rest prioritizes weakly learned information and predicts memory performance

Anna C Schapiro, Elizabeth A McDevitt, Timothy T Rogers, Sara C Mednick, Kenneth A Norman, Anna C Schapiro, Elizabeth A McDevitt, Timothy T Rogers, Sara C Mednick, Kenneth A Norman

Abstract

The hippocampus replays experiences during quiet rest periods, and this replay benefits subsequent memory. A critical open question is how memories are prioritized for this replay. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) pattern analysis to track item-level replay in the hippocampus during an awake rest period after participants studied 15 objects and completed a memory test. Objects that were remembered less well were replayed more during the subsequent rest period, suggesting a prioritization process in which weaker memories-memories most vulnerable to forgetting-are selected for replay. In a second session 12 hours later, more replay of an object during a rest period predicted better subsequent memory for that object. Replay predicted memory improvement across sessions only for participants who slept during that interval. Our results provide evidence that replay in the human hippocampus prioritizes weakly learned information, predicts subsequent memory performance, and relates to memory improvement across a delay with sleep.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Stimuli and design. a Examples of stimuli presented from the three classes Alpha, Beta, and Gamma, labeled with unique code names. Satellites were built randomly for each participant, using the same category structure. b Sessions 1 and 2 procedures for all participants, with delay between sessions either overnight or across day depending on group
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Behavioral results. Change in proportion correct from the first to second session for Sleep and Wake group, for different feature types and the average across feature types; §p < 0.1, *p< 0.05, ***p < 0.001, one-tailed t-test. Asterisks above horizontal lines show significant differences between conditions; asterisks without bars indicate where conditions differ from zero. Gray circles indicate performance of individual subjects. Error bars denote ± 1 SEM; n = 12 in each group
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Replay analysis. a Each satellite’s template consists of a pattern of beta weights across all voxels in the hippocampus. The template is correlated with the preprocessed pattern of activity across all voxels in the hippocampus at each TR of the rest period. b This results in a matrix of correlation values between all templates and all rest period TRs, which is then thresholded to reflect “potential replays”. c The potential replay activity for a given template is summed across TRs and then correlated with memory performance. The inset scatterplot shows data for one representative subject’s Session 1 correlation
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Within-session replay–behavior relationships. a Correlation between memory in Session 1 and replay in Session 1, and between memory in Session 2 and replay in Session 2, in left (L Hipp) and right (R Hipp) hippocampus. b Same data, broken down by Sleep and Wake groups. c Same data, broken down by memory for unique and shared features. d Same data, broken down by memory for verbal and visual features. §p < 0.1, *p< 0.05, **p< 0.01, ***p < 0.001, t-test. Error bars denote ± 1 SEM; n = 24 for all bars except b, where n = 12 for each group
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Across-session replay–behavior relationships. Correlation between replay in each session and improvement in memory from the first to second session, as well as the average for each group across sessions; §p < 0.1, *p< 0.05, **p < 0.01, t-test, one-tailed for comparing Sleep to zero and Sleep to Wake. Error bars denote ± 1 SEM; n = 12 in each group

References

    1. Buckner RL. The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. Neuroimage. 2012;62:1137–1145. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.035.
    1. Carr MF, Jadhav SP, Frank LM. Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:147–153. doi: 10.1038/nn.2732.
    1. O’Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Dupret D, Csicsvari J. Play it again: reactivation of waking experience and memory. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006.
    1. Jadhav SP, Kemere C, German PW, Frank LM. Awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples support spatial memory. Science. 2012;336:1454–1458. doi: 10.1126/science.1217230.
    1. Girardeau G, Zugaro M. Hippocampal ripples and memory consolidation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2011;21:452–459. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.02.005.
    1. van de Ven GM, Trouche S, McNamara CG, Allen K, Dupret D. Hippocampal offline reactivation consolidates recently formed cell assembly patterns during sharp wave-ripples. Neuron. 2016;92:968–974. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.020.
    1. Staresina BP, Alink A, Kriegeskorte N, Henson RN. Awake reactivation predicts memory in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:21159–21164. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311989110.
    1. Deuker L, et al. Memory consolidation by replay of stimulus-specific neural activity. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:19373–19383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0414-13.2013.
    1. Kurth-Nelson Z, Economides M, Dolan RJ, Dayan P. Fast sequences of non-spatial state representations in humans. Neuron. 2016;91:194–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.028.
    1. Tambini A, Davachi L. Persistence of hippocampal multivoxel patterns into postencoding rest is related to memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:19591–19596. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308499110.
    1. Tambini A, Ketz N, Davachi L. Enhanced brain correlations during rest are related to memory for recent experiences. Neuron. 2010;65:280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001.
    1. Schlichting ML, Preston AR. Memory reactivation during rest supports upcoming learning of related content. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:15845–15850. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1404396111.
    1. Schlichting ML, Preston AR. Hippocampal-medial prefrontal circuit supports memory updating during learning and post-encoding rest. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2016;134 Pt A:91–106. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.11.005.
    1. Gruber MJ, Ritchey M, Wang SF, Doss MK, Ranganath C. Post-learning hippocampal dynamics promote preferential retention of rewarding events. Neuron. 2016;89:1110–1120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.017.
    1. Murty VP, Tompary A, Adcock RA, Davachi L. Selectivity in postencoding connectivity with high-level visual cortex is associated with reward-motivated memory. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:537–545. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4032-15.2016.
    1. Tompary A, Duncan K, Davachi L. Consolidation of associative and item memory is related to post-encoding functional connectivity between the ventral tegmental area and different medial temporal lobe subregions during an unrelated task. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:7326–7331. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4816-14.2015.
    1. de Voogd LD, Fernandez G, Hermans EJ. Awake reactivation of emotional memory traces through hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Neuroimage. 2016;134:563–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.026.
    1. Wilhelm I, et al. Sleep selectively enhances memory expected to be of future relevance. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:1563–1569. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-10.2011.
    1. Payne JD, Stickgold R, Swanberg K, Kensinger EA. Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emotional components of scenes. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:781–788. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02157.x.
    1. Drosopoulos S, Schulze C, Fischer S, Born J. Sleep’s function in the spontaneous recovery and consolidation of memories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007;136:169–183. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.169.
    1. Peters KR, Smith V, Smith CT. Changes in sleep architecture following motor learning depend on initial skill level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007;19:817–829. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.817.
    1. Diekelmann S, Born J, Wagner U. Sleep enhances false memories depending on general memory performance. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;208:425–429. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.021.
    1. Cairney SA, Lindsay S, Sobczak JM, Paller KA, Gaskell MG. The benefits of targeted memory reactivation for consolidation in sleep are contingent on memory accuracy and direct cue-memory associations. Sleep. 2016;39:1139–1150. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5772.
    1. McDevitt EA, Duggan KA, Mednick SC. REM sleep rescues learning from interference. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2015;122:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.11.015.
    1. Kuriyama K, Stickgold R, Walker MP. Sleep-dependent learning and motor-skill complexity. Learn. Mem. 2004;11:705–713. doi: 10.1101/lm.76304.
    1. Djonlagic I, et al. Sleep enhances category learning. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:751–755. doi: 10.1101/lm.1634509.
    1. Sio UN, Monaghan P, Ormerod T. Sleep on it, but only if it is difficult: effects of sleep on problem solving. Mem. Cognit. 2013;41:159–166. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0256-7.
    1. Tucker MA, Fishbein W. Enhancement of declarative memory performance following a daytime nap is contingent on strength of initial task acquisition. Sleep. 2008;31:197–203. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.2.197.
    1. Talamini LM, Nieuwenhuis IL, Takashima A, Jensen O. Sleep directly following learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. Learn. Mem. 2008;15:233–237. doi: 10.1101/lm.771608.
    1. Schapiro AC, et al. Sleep benefits memory for semantic category structure while preserving exemplar-specific information. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:14869. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12884-5.
    1. Chen Z, Wilson MA. Deciphering neural codes of memory during sleep. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40:260–275. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.03.005.
    1. Rasch B, Born J. Maintaining memories by reactivation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:698–703. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.007.
    1. Stickgold R, Walker MP. Sleep-dependent memory triage: evolving generalization through selective processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:139–145. doi: 10.1038/nn.3303.
    1. Gregory MD, et al. Resting state connectivity immediately following learning correlates with subsequent sleep-dependent enhancement of motor task performance. Neuroimage. 2014;102 Pt 2:666–673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.044.
    1. Stickgold R. Parsing the role of sleep in memory processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:847–853. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.002.
    1. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990;52:29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241.
    1. Leon AC, Heo M. Sample sizes required to detect interactions between two binary fixed-effects in a mixed-effects linear regression model. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 2009;53:603–608. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2008.06.010.
    1. Schapiro, A. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., Botvinick, M. M. & Norman, K. A. Complementary learning systems within the hippocampus: a neural network modelling approach to reconciling episodic memory with statistical learning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 372, 20160049 (2017).
    1. Wang L, Mruczek RE, Arcaro MJ, Kastner S. Probabilistic maps of visual topography in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:3911–3931. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu277.
    1. Kastner S, et al. Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3494–3507. doi: 10.1152/jn.00010.2007.
    1. Groen G, Sokolov AN, Jonas C, Roebling R, Spitzer M. Increased resting-state perfusion after repeated encoding is related to later retrieval of declarative associative memories. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e19985. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019985.
    1. Gupta AS, van der Meer MA, Touretzky DS, Redish AD. Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience. Neuron. 2010;65:695–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.034.
    1. Jafarpour Anna, Penny Will, Barnes Gareth, Knight Robert T., Duzel Emrah. Working Memory Replay Prioritizes Weakly Attended Events. eneuro. 2017;4(4):ENEURO.0171-17.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0171-17.2017.
    1. Tambini A, Berners-Lee A, Davachi L. Brief targeted memory reactivation during the awake state enhances memory stability and benefits the weakest memories. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:15325. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15608-x.
    1. Schmidt C, et al. Encoding difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:8976–8982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2464-06.2006.
    1. Singer AC, Frank LM. Rewarded outcomes enhance reactivation of experience in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2009;64:910–921. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.016.
    1. Bursley JK, Nestor A, Tarr MJ, Creswell JD. Awake, offline processing during associative learning. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0127522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127522.
    1. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 1995;102:419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419.
    1. Rogers, T. T. & McClelland, J. L. Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2004).
    1. Molle M, Born J. Hippocampus whispering in deep sleep to prefrontal cortex--for good memories? Neuron. 2009;61:496–498. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.002.
    1. Takashima A, et al. Shift from hippocampal to neocortical centered retrieval network with consolidation. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:10087–10093. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0799-09.2009.
    1. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1992;15:376–381. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.4.376.
    1. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110.
    1. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.
    1. Fischl B, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X.
    1. Hindy Nicholas C., Turk-Browne Nicholas B. Action-Based Learning of Multistate Objects in the Medial Temporal Lobe. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;26(5):1853–1865. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv030.

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi