Effects of subjective successful aging on emotional and coping responses to the COVID-19 pandemic

Dannii Y Yeung, Edwin K H Chung, Alfred H K Lam, Alvin K K Ho, Dannii Y Yeung, Edwin K H Chung, Alfred H K Lam, Alvin K K Ho

Abstract

Background: Middle-aged and older adults are more vulnerable to hospitalization and mortality if they are infected with the COVID-19 virus. The present study investigates the longitudinal effects of subjective successful aging on middle-aged and older adults' emotional and coping responses to the COVID-19 pandemic, and explores an underlying mechanism through perceived time limitation during the pandemic.

Methods: A sample of 311 Hong Kong Chinese middle-aged and older adults (Mage = 64.58, SD = 10.14, Range = 45-90 years) were recruited from an Adult Development and Aging Project and participated in a questionnaire study via an online platform or phone interview. Their levels of subjective successful aging, perceived time limitation, and emotional and coping responses to the pandemic were measured.

Results: The respondents who perceived themselves as more successful in aging process reported more positive and fewer negative emotions compared with their counterparts with lower levels of subjective successful aging. The mediation analysis showed that perceived time limitation could partially account for the effects of subjective successful aging on emotional and coping responses.

Conclusions: Findings of this study unveil the beneficial effects of subjective views of successful aging on emotional and coping responses to the pandemic through alleviating their perception of time limitation.

Keywords: Coronavirus disease; Emotional and coping responses; Perceived time limitation; Subjective perceptions of successful aging.

Conflict of interest statement

Dannii Yeung is an Editorial Board member for BMC Geriatrics. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
The mediation model on the relationships between subjective successful aging and five outcome variables through perceived time limitation. Note. Numbers in the figure indicate unstandardized regression coefficients. c’ denotes the direct effect of subjective successful aging on each outcome variable. The numbers in the parentheses represent the indirect effect of subjective successful aging on the outcome variable through perceived time limitation. The solid lines denote significant associations between variables at p < .05, whereas dashed lines represent non-significant associations. Age, sex, education level, work status, depression, perceived severity, and level of reduced social contact were statistically controlled as covariates in the mediation analysis. Goodness of fit of this mediation model: χ2 = 14.58 (df = 7, p = .04), CFI = .98, RMSEA = .06, SRMR = .03. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001

References

    1. Hong Kong SAR Government. 2020. Prevention and control of disease (requirement and dmirections) (business and premises) regulation gazetted. . Accessed 31 May 2020.
    1. Kwok KO, Li KK, Chan HH, Yi YY, Tang A, Wei WI, et al. Community responses during early phase of COVID-19 epidemic. Hong Kong Emerg Infect Dis. 2020;26. 10.3201/eid2607.200500.
    1. Hong Kong SAR Government . Latest situation of cases of COVID-19 (as of 17 May 2020) 2020.
    1. Bowling A. Aspirations for older age in the 21st century: what is successful aging? Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2007;64(3):263–297. doi: 10.2190/L0K1-87W4-9R01-7127.
    1. Cosco TD, Prina AM, Perales J, Stephan BCM, Brayne C. Operational definitions of successful aging: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014;26(3):373–381. doi: 10.1017/S1041610213002287.
    1. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging. New York: Dell Publishing; 1998.
    1. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging. Gerontologist. 1997;37(4):433–440. doi: 10.1093/geront/37.4.433.
    1. Feng Q, Straughan PT. What does successful aging mean? Lay perception of successful aging among elderly Singaporeans. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72(2):204–213. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw151.
    1. Nakagawa T, Cho J, Yeung DY. Successful aging in East Asia: comparison among China, Korea, and Japan. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020. 10.1093/geronb/gbaa042.
    1. Katz S, Calasanti T. Critical perspectives on successful aging: does it “appeal more than it illuminates”? Gerontologist. 2015;55(1):26–33. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu027.
    1. Martinson M, Berridge C. Successful aging and its discontents: a systematic review of social gerontology literature. Gerontologist. 2015;55(1):58–69. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu037.
    1. Ingrand I, Paccalin M, Liuu E, Gil R, Ingrand P. Positive perception of aging is a key predictor of quality-of-life in aging people. PLoS One. 2018;13(10):e0204044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204044.
    1. Pruchno RA, Wilson-Genderson M, Cartwright F. A two-factor model of successful aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65(6):671–679. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq051.
    1. Reichstadt J, Sengupta G, Depp CA, Palinkas LA, Jeste DV. Older adults’ perspectives on successful aging: qualitative interviews. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(7):567–575. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181e040bb.
    1. Hsu HC, Jones BL. Multiple trajectories of successful aging of older and younger cohorts. Gerontologist. 2012;52(6):843–856. doi: 10.1093/geront/gns005.
    1. Østbye T, Krause KM, Norton MC, Tschanz J, Sanders L, Hayden K, et al. Ten dimensions of health and their relationships with overall self-reported health and survival in a predominately religiously active elderly population: the cache county memory study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54(2):199–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.00583.x.
    1. Montross LP, Depp C, Daly J, Reichstadt J, Golshan S, Moore D, et al. Correlates of self-rated successful aging among community-dwelling older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):43–51. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192489.43179.31.
    1. Feng Q, Son J, Zeng Y. Prevalence and correlates of successful ageing: a comparison between China and South Korea. Eur J Ageing. 2015;12:83–94. doi: 10.1007/s10433-014-0329-5.
    1. Stewart JM, Auais M, Belanger E, Phillips SP. Comparison of self-rated and objective successful ageing in an international cohort. Ageing Soc. 2019;39(7):1317–1334. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X17001489.
    1. Strawbridge WJ, Wallhagen MI, Cohen RD. Successful aging and well-being: self-rated compared with Rowe and Kahn. Gerontologist. 2002;42(6):727–733. doi: 10.1093/geront/42.6.727.
    1. Cernin PA, Lysack C, Lichtenberg PA. A comparison of self-rated and objectively measured successful aging constructs in an urban sample of African American older adults. Clin Gerontol. 2011;34(2):89–102. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2011.539525.
    1. Levy BR, Slade MD, Kunkel SR, Kasl SV. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83(2):261–270. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.261.
    1. Dutt AJ, Gabrian M, Wahl H-W. Developmental regulation and awareness of age-related change: a (mostly) unexplored connection. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2018;73(6):934–943. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw084.
    1. Brassen S, Gamer M, Peters J, Gluth S, Büchel C. Don’t look back in anger! Responsiveness to missed chances in successful and nonsuccessful aging. Science. 2012;336(6081):612–614. doi: 10.1126/science.1217516.
    1. Wrosch C, Scheier MF, Miller GE, Schulz R, Carver CS. Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29(12):1494–1508. doi: 10.1177/0146167203256921.
    1. Kahana E, Kahana B. Contextualizing successful aging: new directions in an age-old search. In: Settersten RJ, editor. Invitation to the life course: toward new understanding of later life. New York: Baywood Publishing Company, Inc; 2003.
    1. Ouwehand C, de Riddle DTD, Bensing JM. A review of successful aging models: proposing proactive coping as an important additional strategy. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(8):873–884. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.11.003.
    1. Fung HH, Carstensen LL. Goals change when life’s fragility is primed: lessons learned from older adults, the September 11 attacks and SARS. Soc Cogn. 2006;24(3):248–278. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.3.248.
    1. Gabrian M, Dutt AJ, Wahl H-W. Subjective time perceptions and aging well: a review of concepts and empirical research—a mini-review. Gerontology. 2017;63(4):350–358. doi: 10.1159/000470906.
    1. Carstensen LL. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science. 2006;312(5782):1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488.
    1. Fung HH, Carstensen LL. Motivational changes in response to blocked goals and foreshortened time: testing alternatives to socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychol Aging. 2004;19(1):68–78. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.68.
    1. Fung HH, Carstensen LL, Lutz AM. Influence of time on social preferences: implications for life-span development. Psychol Aging. 1999;14(4):595–604. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595.
    1. Hoppmann CA, Infurna FJ, Ram N, Gerstorf D. Associations among individuals’ perceptions of future time, individual resources, and subjective well-being in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72(3):388–399. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv063.
    1. Yeung DY, Ho AK-K. Focus on opportunities or limitations? Their effects on older workers’ conflict management strategies. Front Psychol. 2020;11:571874. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.571874.
    1. Yeung DY, Fung HH, Kam C. Age differences in problem solving strategies: the mediating role of future time perspective. Pers Individ Differ. 2012;53(1):38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.02.014.
    1. Shrira A, Hoffman Y, Bodner E, Palgi Y. COVID-19 related loneliness and psychiatric symptoms among older adults: the buffering role of subjective age. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020;28(11):1200–1204. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.05.018.
    1. Yeung DY, Fung HH. Age differences in coping and emotional responses toward SARS: a longitudinal study of Hong Kong Chinese. Aging Ment Health. 2007;11(5):579–587. doi: 10.1080/13607860601086355.
    1. Losada-Baltar A, Jiménez-Gonzalo L, Gallego-Alberto L, MDS P-C, Fernandes-Pires J, Márquez-González M. “We’re staying at home”. Association of self-perceptions of aging, personal and family resources and loneliness with psychological distress during the lock-down period of COVID-19. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020. 10.1093/geronb/gbaa048.
    1. Krendl AC, Perry BL. The impact of sheltering in place during the COVID-19 pandemic on older adults’ social and mental well-being. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020. 10.1093/geronb/gbaa110.
    1. Skapinakis P, Bellos S, Oikonomou A, Dimitriadis G, Gkikas P, Perdikari E, et al. Depression and its relationship with coping strategies and illness perceptions during the COVID-19 lockdown in Greece: a cross-sectional survey of the population. Depress Res Treat. 2020. 10.1155/2020/3158954.
    1. Dawson DL, Golijani-Moghaddam N. COVID-19: psychological flexibility, coping, mental health, and wellbeing in the UK during the pandemic. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2020;17:126–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.07.010.
    1. Hong Kong SAR Government . Prevention and control of disease (prohibition on gmroup gathering) regulation. 2020.
    1. Fung HH, Yeung DY, Li K-K, Lang FR. Benefits of negative social exchanges for emotional closeness. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64(5):612–621. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp065.
    1. Hong Kong SAR Government . Entry restriction on non-Hong Kong residents coming from overseas extended [press release] 2020.
    1. Chung EKH, Lam A, Yeung DY. Health-promoting behaviours and psychological well-being: the mediating role of sense of control. Manuscript under review. 2021.
    1. Yeung DY, Fung HH, Chan DK-S. Roles of age and future time perspective of the work relationship in conflict management: a daily diary study. Int J Stress Manag. 2020;27(4):358–369. doi: 10.1037/str0000155.
    1. Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the brief COPE. Int J Behav Med. 1997;4(1):92–100. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6.
    1. Carstensen LL, Lang FR. Future time perspective scale [unpublished manuscript] Stanford, CA: Located at: Stanford University; 1995.
    1. Yeung DY, Fung HH, Lang FR. Gender differences in social network characteristics and psychological well-being among Hong Kong Chinese: the role of future time perspective and adherence to Renqing. Aging Ment Health. 2007;11(1):45–56. doi: 10.1080/13607860600735820.
    1. Chan SM, Kwok WW, Fung TWT. Psychometric properties of the Zimbardo time perspective inventory in Hong Kong adolescents. Time Soc. 2019;28(1):33–49. doi: 10.1177/0961463X16678250.
    1. MacLeod AK, Salaminiou E. Reduced positive future-thinking in depression: cognitive and affective factors. Cogn Emot. 2001;15(1):99–107. doi: 10.1080/02699930125776.
    1. Thimm JC, Holte A, Brennen T, Wang CEA. Hope and expectancies for future events in depression. Front Psychol. 2013;4:470. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00470.
    1. Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. Am J Prev Med. 1994;10(2):77–84. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30622-6.
    1. Boey KW. Cross-validation of a short form of the CES-D in Chinese elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1999;14(8):608–617. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199908)14:8<608::AID-GPS991>;2-Z.
    1. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02.
    1. World Health Organization . COVID-19: Vulnerable and high risk groups. 2020.
    1. Levy BR, Myers LM. Preventive health behaviors influenced by self-perceptions of aging. Prev Med. 2004;39(3):625–629. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.02.029.
    1. Folkman S, Lazarus RS. An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. J Health Soc Behav. 1980;21(3):219–239. doi: 10.2307/2136617.
    1. Cheng C. Assessing coping flexibility in real-life and laboratory settings: a multimethod approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80(5):814–833. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.80.5.814.
    1. Ho HCY, Yeung DY. Effects of occupational future time perspective on managing stressful work situations. Int J Psychol. 2016;51(4):261–268. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12144.
    1. Rohr MK, John DT, Fung HH, Lang FR. A three-component model of future time perspective across adulthood. Psychol Aging. 2017;32(7):597–607. doi: 10.1037/pag0000191.
    1. Tovel H, Carmel S. Maintaining successful aging: the role of coping patterns and resources. J Happiness Stud. 2014;15(2):255–270. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9420-4.
    1. Mellor D, Hayashi Y, Firth L, Stokes M, Chambers S, Cummins R. Volunteering and well-being: do self-esteem, optimism, and perceived control mediate the relationship? J Soc Serv Res. 2008;34(4):61–70. doi: 10.1080/01488370802162483.
    1. Caldwell RL. At the confluence of memory and meaning—life review with older adults and families: using narrative therapy and the expressive arts to re-member and re-author stories of resilience. Fam J. 2005;13(2):172–175. doi: 10.1177/1066480704273338.
    1. Cohen A. Welfare clients' volunteering as a means of empowerment. Nonprofit Volunt Sect Q. 2009;38(3):522–534. doi: 10.1177/0899764008320196.
    1. Cate RA, John OP. Testing models of the structure and development of future time perspective: maintaining a focus on opportunities in middle age. Psychol Aging. 2007;22(1):186–201. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.186.

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi