Culture, carrying, and communication: Beliefs and behavior associated with babywearing

Emily E Little, Cristine H Legare, Leslie J Carver, Emily E Little, Cristine H Legare, Leslie J Carver

Abstract

Ethnographic research suggests mother-infant physical contact predicts high levels of maternal responsiveness to infant cues, yet it is unclear whether this responsiveness is driven by the act of physical contact or by underlying beliefs about responsiveness. We examine beliefs and behavior associated with infant carrying (i.e., babywearing) among U.S. mothers and experimentally test the effect of mother-infant physical contact on maternal responsiveness. In Study 1 (N = 23 dyads), babywearing mothers were more likely to interact contingently in response to infant cues than non-babywearing mothers during an in-lab play session. In Study 2 (N = 492 mothers), babywearing predicted maternal beliefs emphasizing responsiveness to infant cues. In Study 3 (N = 20 dyads), we experimentally manipulated mother-infant physical contact in the lab using a within-subjects design and found that babywearing increased maternal tactile interaction, decreased maternal and infant object contact, and increased maternal responsiveness to infant vocalizations. Our results motivate further research examining how culturally-mediated infant carrying practices shape the infant's early social environment and subsequent development.

Keywords: Babywearing; Maternal responsiveness; Mother-infant interaction; Physical contact; Touch.

Copyright © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Experimental setup in Study 1 for both babywearers and non-babywearers (A), as well as the physical condition (B) and visual condition (C) in Study 3.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Mean difference in overall maternal responsiveness between mothers with less experience with physical contact and more experience with physical contact in Study 1. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Mean difference in proximal care belief score associated with infant transport method in Study 2. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Difference in maternal responses to infants’ vocalizations when not in physical contact with their infant and when in physical contact (within-subjects) in Study 3. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

References

    1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, & Wall SN (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    1. Akhtar N, & Gernsbacher MA (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 59–65. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.x.
    1. Altmann J, & Samuels A. (1992). Costs of maternal care: Infant-carrying in baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29(6), 391–398.
    1. Anisfeld E, Casper V, Nozyce M, & Cunningham N. (1990). Does infant carrying promote attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the development of attachment. Child Development, 61(5), 1617–1627. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02888.x.
    1. Arnett JJ (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. The American Psychologist, 63(7), 602. 10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602.
    1. Bard K. (2002). Primate parenting. 2002 In Bornstein M. (Ed.). Handbook of parenting: Second edition: Volume 2 biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 99–140). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    1. Barr RG, Konner M, Bakeman R, & Adamson L. (1991). Crying in! Kung San infants: A test of the cultural specificity hypothesis. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 33(7), 601–610. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14930.x.
    1. Bigelow AE, & Birch SA (1999). The effects of contingency in previous interactions on infants’ preference for social partners. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(3), 367–382. 10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00016-8.
    1. Bigelow AE, & Power M. (2012). The effect of mother–infant skin-to-skin contact on infants’ response to the still face task from newborn to three months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(2), 240–251.
    1. Bigelow AE, Littlejohn M, Bergman N, & McDonald C. (2010). The relation between early mother–infant skin-to-skin contact and later maternal sensitivity in South African mothers of low birth weight infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31(3), 358–377.
    1. Bigelow A, Power M, MacLellan-Peters J, Alex M, & McDonald C. (2012). Effect of mother/infant skin-to-skin contact on postpartum depressive symptoms and maternal physiological stress. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic & Neonatal Nursing, 41(3), 369–382.
    1. Bigelow AE, Power M, Gillis DE, Maclellan-Peters J, Alex M, & McDonald C. (2014). Breastfeeding, skin-to-skin contact, and mother-infant interactions over infants’ first three months. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(1), 51–62.
    1. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Tal J, Ludemann P, Toda S, Rahn CW, et al. (1992). Maternal responsiveness to infants in three societies: The United States, France, and Japan. Child Development, 63, 808–821. 10.2307/1131235.
    1. Bornstein MH, Cote LR, & Venuti P. (2001). Parenting beliefs and behaviors in northern and southern groups of Italian mothers of young infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(4), 663. 10.1037//0893-32O0.15.4.663.
    1. Broesch T, Rochat P, Olah K, Broesch J, & Henrich J. (2016). Similarities and Differences in maternal responsiveness in three societies: Evidence from Fiji, Kenya, and the United States. Child Development, 87(3), 700–711. 10.1111/cdev.12501.
    1. Bystrova K, Ivanova V, Edhborg M, Matthiesen AS, Ransjö-Arvidson AB, Mukhamedrakhimov R, et al. (2009). Early contact versus separation: Effects on mother–infant interaction one year later. Birth, 36(2), 97–109. 10.1111/j.1523-536X.2009.00307.x.
    1. Caudill WA, & Schooler C. (1973). Child behavior and child rearing in Japan and the United States: An interim report. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157(5), 323–338.
    1. Charpak N, Ruiz-Peláez JG, & Charpak Y. (2001). A randomized, controlled trial of kangaroo mother care: Results of follow-up at 1 year of corrected age. Pediatrics, 108(5), 1072–1079. 10.1542/peds.108.5.1072.
    1. Chwo MJ, Anderson GC, Good M, Dowling DA, Shiau SHH, & Chu DM (2002). A randomized controlled trial of early kangaroo care for preterm infants: Effects on temperature, weight, behavior, and acuity. Journal of Nursing Research, 10(2), 129–142. 10.1097/01.JNR.0000347592.43768.46.
    1. De Wolff MS, & Van Ijzendoorn MH (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68(4), 571–591. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x.
    1. Dettwyler KA (1988). More than nutrition: Breastfeeding in urban Mali. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 2(2), 172–183. 10.1525/maq.1988.2.2.02a00060.
    1. Dunst CJ, & Kassow DZ (2008). Caregiver sensitivity, contingent social responsiveness, and secure infant attachment. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 5(1), 40. 10.1037/h0100409.
    1. Feldman R, Eidelman AI, Sirota L, & Weller A. (2002). Comparison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: Parenting outcomes and preterm infant development. Pediatrics, 110(1), 16–26. 10.1542/peds.110.1.16.
    1. Ferber SG, Kuint J, Weller A, Feldman R, Dollberg S, Arbel E, et al. (2002). Massage therapy by mothers and trained professionals enhances weight gain in preterm infants. Early Human Development, 67(1), 37–45. 10.1016/S0378-3782(01)00249-3.
    1. Gettler LT (2010). Direct male care and hominin evolution: Why male-child interaction is more than a nice social idea. American Anthropologist, 112(1), 7–21.
    1. Gettler LT, McKenna JJ, Agustin SS, McDade TW, & Kuzawa CW (2012). Does cosleeping contribute to lower testosterone levels in fathers? Evidence from the Philippines. PLoS ONE, 7(9), 41559.
    1. Goldstein MH, & Schwade JA (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515–523. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x.
    1. Goldstein MH, Schwade JA, & Bornstein MH (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child development, 80(3), 636–644. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01287.x.
    1. Granju KA, & Kennedy B. (1999). Attachment parenting: Instinctive care for your baby and young child. Simon and Schuster.
    1. Gros-Louis J, West MJ, & King AP (2016). The influence of interactive context on prelinguistic vocalizations and maternal responses. Language Learning and Development, 12(3), 280–294. 10.1080/15475441.2015.1053563.
    1. Harkness S, & Super CM (2006). Themes and variations: Parental ethnotheories in Western cultures. Parenting Beliefs, Behaviors, and Parent-Child Relations: A CrossCultural Perspective, 61–79.
    1. Harwood RL, Schoelmerich A, Schulze PA, & Gonzalez Z. (1999). Cultural differences in maternal beliefs and behaviors: A study of middle-class Anglo and Puerto Rican mother-infant pairs in four everyday situations. Child Development, 70(4), 1005–1016. 10.1111/1467-8624.00073.
    1. Henrich J, Heine SJ, & Norenzayan A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29. 10.1038/466029a.
    1. Hewlett BS, & Lamb ME (2002). 11 Integrating evolution, culture and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant proximity and responsiveness in central Africa and the USA. In Keller H, Poortinga YH, & Schoelmerich A. (Eds.). Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 241–269). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Hewlett BS, Lamb ME, Shannon D, Leyendecker B, & Schölmerich A. (1998). Culture and early infancy among central African foragers and farmers. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 653. 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.653.
    1. Hrdy SB (2011). Mothers and others. Harvard University Press.
    1. Hunziker UA, & Barr RG (1986). Increased carrying reduces infant crying: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 77(5), 641–648.
    1. Kärtner J, Keller H, Lamm B, Abels M, Yovsi RD, Chaudhary N, et al. (2008). Similarities and differences in contingency experiences of 3-month-olds across sociocultural contexts. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(3), 488–500. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.01.001.
    1. Kärtner J, Keller H, & Yovsi RD (2010). Mother–infant interaction during the first 3 months: The emergence of culture-specific contingency patterns. Child Development, 81, 540–554. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01414.x.
    1. Keller H. (2002). Development as the interface between biology and culture: A conceptualization of early ontogenetic experiences. In Keller H, Poortinga YH, & Schoelmerich A. (Eds.). Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 215–240). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Keller H, Yovsi R, Borke J, Kärtner J, Jensen H, & Papaligoura Z. (2004). Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: Self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. Child Development, 75(6), 1745–1760. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00814.x.
    1. Keller H, Borke J, Staufenbiel T, Yovsi RD, Abels M, Papaligoura Z, et al. (2009). Distal and proximal parenting as alternative parenting strategies during infants’ early months of life: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(5), 412–420. 10.1177/0165025409338441.
    1. Lamm B, & Keller H. (2007). Understanding cultural models of parenting: The role of intracultural variation and response style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(1), 50–57.
    1. Lausberg H, & Sloetjes H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3), 841–849.
    1. Leonard WR, & Robertson ML (1992). Nutritional requirements and human evolution: A bioenergetics model. American Journal of Human Biology, 4(2), 179–195.
    1. Little EE, Carver LJ, & Legare CH (2016). Cultural variation in triadic infant caregiver object exploration. Child Development, 87(4), 1130–1145. 10.1111/cdev.12513.
    1. Little E, Legare C, & Carver L. (2018). Mother–infant physical contact predicts responsive feeding among U.S. breastfeeding mothers. Nutrients, 10(9), 1251. 10.3390/nu10091251.
    1. Maudlin JG, Sandlin JA, & Thaller J. (2012). Baby culture and the curriculum of consumption: A critical reading of the film Babies. Pedagogy, Culture & Society, 20(2), 211–229. 10.1080/14681366.2012.674053.
    1. McKenna JJ, & Gettler LT (2016). There is no such thing as infant sleep, there is no such thing as breastfeeding, there is only breastsleeping. Acta Paediatrica, 105(1), 17–21. 10.1111/apa.13161.
    1. Mesman J, Minter T, Angnged A, Cissé IA, Salali GD, & Migliano AB (2017). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89(3), 837–850. 10.1111/cdev.12795.
    1. Moore ER, Anderson GC, Bergman N, & Dowswell T. (2012). Early skin-to-skin contact for mothers and their healthy newborn infants. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews, 5, CD003519.
    1. Nicely P, Tamis-LeMonda CS, & Bornstein MH (1999). Mothers’ attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(4), 557–568. 10.1016/S0163-6383(00)00023-0.
    1. Nielsen M, Haun D, Kärtner J, & Legare CH (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 31–38. 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017.
    1. Pisacane A, Continisio P, Filosa C, Tagliamonte V, & Continisio GI (2012). Use of baby carriers to increase breastfeeding duration among term infants: The effects of an educational intervention in Italy. Acta Paediatrica, 101(10), e434–e438. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02758.x.
    1. Richman AL, Miller PM, & LeVine RA (1992). Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 614. 10.1037/00121649.28.4.614.
    1. Sears W, & Sears M. (2001). The attachment parenting book: A commonsense guide to understanding and nurturing your baby. Little, Brown..
    1. Shwalb DW, Shwalb BJ, & Shoji J. (1996). Japanese mothers’ ideas about infants and temperament. Parents’ Cultural Belief Systems: Their Origins, Expressions, and Consequences, 161–191.
    1. St James-Roberts I, Alvarez M, Csipke E, Abramsky T, Goodwin J, & Sorgenfrei E. (2006). Infant crying and sleeping in London, Copenhagen and when parents adopt a “proximal” form of care. Pediatrics, 117(6), 10.1542/peds.2005-2387.
    1. Stevens E, Blake J, Vitale G, & Macdonald S. (1998). Mother-infant object involvement at 9 and 15 months: Relation to infant cognition and early vocabulary. First Language, 18(53), 203–222. 10.1177/014272379801805305.
    1. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, & Song L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 121–126. 10.1177/0963721414522813.
    1. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, & Baumwell L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748–767. 10.1111/1467-8624.00313.
    1. Wall-Scheffler CM, & Myers MJ (2008). Working harder and taking longer: How frontal loads can impact female reproductive costs. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 46(Suppl), 217.
    1. Wall-Scheffler CM, Geiger K, & Steudel-Numbers KL (2007). Infant carrying: The role of increased locomotory costs in early tool development. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 133(2), 841–846. 10.1002/ajpa.20603.

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi