Resolving the Spatial Profile of Figure Enhancement in Human V1 through Population Receptive Field Modeling

Sonia Poltoratski, Frank Tong, Sonia Poltoratski, Frank Tong

Abstract

The detection and segmentation of meaningful figures from their background is one of the primary functions of vision. While work in nonhuman primates has implicated early visual mechanisms in this figure-ground modulation, neuroimaging in humans has instead largely ascribed the processing of figures and objects to higher stages of the visual hierarchy. Here, we used high-field fMRI at 7 Tesla to measure BOLD responses to task-irrelevant orientation-defined figures in human early visual cortex (N = 6, four females). We used a novel population receptive field mapping-based approach to resolve the spatial profiles of two constituent mechanisms of figure-ground modulation: a local boundary response, and a further enhancement spanning the full extent of the figure region that is driven by global differences in features. Reconstructing the distinct spatial profiles of these effects reveals that figure enhancement modulates responses in human early visual cortex in a manner consistent with a mechanism of automatic, contextually driven feedback from higher visual areas.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT A core function of the visual system is to parse complex 2D input into meaningful figures. We do so constantly and seamlessly, both by processing information about visible edges and by analyzing large-scale differences between figure and background. While influential neurophysiology work has characterized an intriguing mechanism that enhances V1 responses to perceptual figures, we have a poor understanding of how the early visual system contributes to figure-ground processing in humans. Here, we use advanced computational analysis methods and high-field human fMRI data to resolve the distinct spatial profiles of local edge and global figure enhancement in the early visual system (V1 and LGN); the latter is distinct and consistent with a mechanism of automatic, stimulus-driven feedback from higher-level visual areas.

Keywords: LGN; early visual system; fMRI; figure–ground; pRF modeling; perception.

Copyright © 2020 the authors.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Figure–ground modulation in the wild and in the literature. A, An example of animal camouflage, which is pervasive in the natural visual world. Detection of figures is much more difficult when they resemble the color, luminance, and spatial frequency content of the surrounding background. B, A sample orientation-defined figure display, typically shown to an animal such that the receptive field of the recorded neuron falls over the figure region. C, Proposed mechanisms of figure–ground modulation in the primate V1: after an initial visual response (40 ms), local boundary detection mechanisms enhance responses near the figure–surround boundary (60 ms). Subsequently (90 ms), the entire figure region is enhanced. D, The contribution of intrareal horizontal inhibition and excitatory feedback from higher-level visual areas to boundary detection (left) and figure enhancement (right). Adapted from Self et al., (2013) with permission from Elsevier.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Boundary responses and figure enhancement as a function of eccentricity. A, Examples of spatially filtered oriented-noise stimulus displays used in the experiment 2. The following three main conditions are depicted: left, incongruent figure, in which the figure and surround were orthogonally oriented, creating both a perceptual border and an orientation difference; middle, congruent figure, in which the figure and surround are iso-oriented but sampled from distinct patches of noise, creating a visible phase-defined border but a weaker figure percept; right, ground-only condition, in which a single oriented texture fills the visual field. The spatial frequency depicted here is much lower than the experimental stimuli (0.5–8 cycles/°) to improve visibility. See also Movie 1. B, V1 responses plotted using mean beta weights for each experimental condition as a function of the eccentricity of the pRF center of each voxel. Dotted line at 2° eccentricity indicates the figure–surround border; error bars depict ±1 SEM across voxels.. C, V1 BOLD beta weight difference between congruent figure and ground-only conditions, associated with a predicted boundary response. D, V1 BOLD beta weight difference between incongruent and congruent figure conditions, associated with a predicted figure enhancement due to differences in orientation. The spatial profile of this effect as a function of eccentricity is clearly distinct from that of the top panel and appears to extend through the full eccentricity range of the figure but to decline beyond the 2° figure–surround border.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Behavioral performance in the main experiment. A, Behavioral hit rate relative to the onset and offset of 16 s stimulus blocks, plotted in 200 ms increments. Performance is averaged across blocks; shaded error bars indicate ±SEM across subjects. There is no apparent difference between task performance when the stimulus is on the screen versus off, save for a slight dip immediately after the stimulus block that is likely attributable to increased blinking. B, Average hit rate for targets coinciding with the three main experimental condition stimulus blocks, and stimulus-off blocks. Error bars indicate ±SEM across subjects, and grayscale lines correspond to individual participants' performance (some overlap). There is no significant effect of stimulus condition on performance of the fixation task (F(3,20) = 0.46, p = 0.72), nor is there an overall difference between stimulus-on and stimulus-off performance (t(5) = 0.59, p = 0.58). While the fixation task in these experiments is not particularly challenging, we would expect some variation in performance if the task-irrelevant figures were actively or preferentially attracting subjects' attention.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Details of the pRF mapping and binning procedure used in V1 analyses of Figure 2. A, Estimated pRF size (as measured by Gaussian FWHM) plotted as a function of eccentricity for all voxels used in the analyses for the main experiment. pRF size increases both as a function of eccentricity and as one ascends the visual hierarchy from V1 to V3. Inset, Total pRF coverage of V1 across subjects; blue dots mark pRF centers, and gray outlines mark FWHM. Blue circles indicate the figure location (2° eccentricity) and the full display extent (4.5° eccentricity). B, Scatterplot of voxel pRF size (Gaussian FWHM) as a function of eccentricity, with gray lines indicating bin edges. Bins were 0.25° wide with the exception of the most foveal, which included all voxels with a center pRF eccentricity <0.75°. C, Goodness-of-fit of the pRF model across voxels in each bin. Error bars indicate ±SEM across subjects. D, Number of voxels in each bin, across subjects. E, Proportion of voxels in each bin that overlap with the boundary (as defined in Fig. 5A), indicating clear differentiation of voxels in the figure, boundary, and surround regions.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Boundary response and figure enhancement in voxels centered within the figure, plotted as a function of pRF extension beyond boundary. A, As illustrated, two pRFs with centers at the same eccentricity within the figure might, by virtue of differences in their size, lead to differential sensitivity to the location of the figure–surround boundary. We calculated whether the central FWHM region of each pRF fell short of the boundary (negative overlap values) or extended past the boundary (positive overlap values). B, V1 boundary responses and figure enhancement as a function of distance; while boundary responses (congruent minus ground-only condition, gray) is evident primarily in voxels whose pRFs overlap with the figure/surround boundary (overlap >0), figure enhancement (incongruent minus congruent, blue) occurs along the full extent of measured distances. Bins are 0.25° wide, and those containing <10 data points were trimmed. C, Results from a control experiment using 6° diameter figures. While boundary responses again primarily occur in voxels whose pRFs overlap with the boundary (overlap <0), figure enhancement persists even in voxels with up to 2° spatial separation with the figure–surround boundary.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Average BOLD responses across figure, boundary, and surround-selective voxels. A, V1 pRF locations of one representative subject (circles, FWHM), illustrating how pRFs were sorted to create ROIs with voxels primarily responsive to the figure (orange, 205 voxels across participants), surround (purple, 293 voxels), or on the boundary (magenta, 476 voxels). Dotted lines denote the spatial extent of our mapping stimulus (9° diameter) and the figure region (4° diameter). B, V1 results for these ROIs; left panels depict average BOLD time courses in each condition, with dotted lines marking the onset and offset of 16 s stimulus blocks. Right panels depict averaged estimated GLM beta weights in each condition. Error bars indicate ±1 SEM between subjects, and asterisks indicate significance at p < .05, n.s. = no significance. C, Results from voxels in the LGN that overlapped with the center, surround, and boundary.
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
pRF-based visualization of voxelwise BOLD responses in stimulus space. A, Schematic of the multivariate regression-based method of estimation, which allows us to estimate the spatial profile of the observed BOLD effect (yellow) from the measured pRFs (red) and BOLD response (green) of each voxel. The resulting estimate of the spatial profile (yellow) is in the form of a vector of length n2, which is then reshaped to yield images of size n × n like those in B. The precise formula used appears in the Materials and Methods. B, The projected spatial profiles of the differential responses associated with boundary response (congruent minus ground-only condition, left) and figure enhancement (incongruent minus congruent) for V1 data in the main experiment (middle) and in a control experiment which introduced a 0.5° gap between figure and surround (right). Each pixel corresponds to 0.25° of visual angle of the original stimulus space; color depicts predictor values, normalized and averaged across subject. See also Figure 9.
Figure 8.
Figure 8.
A–C, Stimuli (A) and results (B, C) of control experiment A, in which a 0.5° grayscale gap separated the figure and surround. Plotting follows conventions of Figure 2; here, since both conditions have a visible boundary, we can subtract responses of the congruent figure condition (green) from the incongruent figure condition (red) to yield orientation-dependent figure enhancement. Results closely follow the pattern of the main experiment, with the observed enhancement extending throughout the figure region.
Figure 9.
Figure 9.
A comparison of two methods for visualization of the boundary response (congruent minus baseline) in areas V1, V2, and V3. The top panel shows our ridge regression-based approach, while the bottom panel is a simpler average of each pRF weighted by the BOLD amplitude of boundary response-associated BOLD beta weight difference in that voxel. The regression-based approach generally led to better resolved reconstructions, particularly in higher visual areas, which typically have larger pRFs.

References

    1. Aghajari S, Vinke LN, Ling S (2020) Population spatial frequency tuning in human early visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 123:773–785. 10.1152/jn.00291.2019
    1. Alitto HJ, Usrey WM (2008) Origin and dynamics of extraclassical suppression in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the macaque monkey. Neuron 57:135–146. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.019
    1. Anderson BA, Yantis S (2013) Persistence of value-driven attentional capture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39:6–9. 10.1037/a0030860
    1. Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2011) Value-driven attentional capture. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:10367–10371. 10.1073/pnas.1104047108
    1. Appelbaum LG, Wade AR, Vildavski VY, Pettet MW, Norcia AM (2006) Cue-invariant networks for figure and background processing in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 26:11695–11708. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2741-06.2006
    1. Appelbaum LG, Wade AR, Pettet MW, Vildavski VY, Norcia AM (2008) Figure-ground interaction in the human visual cortex. J Vis 8(9):8, 1–19. 10.1167/8.9.8
    1. Bair W, Cavanaugh JR, Movshon JA (2003) Time course and time-distance relationships for surround suppression in macaque V1 neurons. J Neurosci 23:7690–7701. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-20-07690.2003
    1. Bergen JR, Adelson EH (1988) Early vision and texture perception. Nature 333:363–364. 10.1038/333363a0
    1. Bijanzadeh M, Nurminen L, Merlin S, Clark AM, Angelucci A (2018) Distinct laminar processing of local and global context in primate primary visual cortex. Neuron 100:259–274.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.020
    1. Blakemore C, Tobin EA (1972) Lateral inhibition between orientation detectors in the cat's visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 15:439–440. 10.1007/bf00234129
    1. Briggs F, Usrey WM (2008) Emerging views of corticothalamic function. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:403–407. 10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.002
    1. Briggs F, Usrey WM (2011) Corticogeniculate feedback and visual processing in the primate. J Physiol 589:33–40. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.193599
    1. Cohen EH, Tong F (2015) Neural mechanisms of object-based attention. Cereb Cortex 25:1080–1092. 10.1093/cercor/bht303
    1. Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA (2008) Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 39:647–660. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034
    1. Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA (1997) Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7:181–192. 10.1093/cercor/7.2.181
    1. Ghodrati M, Khaligh-Razavi S-M, Lehky SR (2017) Towards building a more complex view of the lateral geniculate nucleus: recent advances in understanding its role. Prog Neurobiol 156:214–255. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.06.002
    1. Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1999) Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron 24:187–203. 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80832-6
    1. Hallum LE, Landy MS, Heeger DJ (2011) Human primary visual cortex (V1) is selective for second-order spatial frequency. J Neurophysiol 105:2121–2131. 10.1152/jn.01007.2010
    1. Hastings C Jr, Mosteller F, Tukey JW (1947) Low moments for small samples: a comparative study of order statistics. Ann Math Statist 8:413–426. 10.1214/aoms/1177730388
    1. Henriksson L, Nurminen L, Hyvärinen A, Vanni S (2008) Spatial frequency tuning in human retinotopic visual areas. J Vis 8(10):5, 1–13. 10.1167/8.10.5
    1. Himmelberg MM, Wade AR (2019) Eccentricity-dependent temporal contrast tuning in human visual cortex measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 184:462–474. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.049
    1. Hoerl AE, Kennard RW (1970) Ridge regression: biased estimation for nonorthogonal problems. J Technometrics 12:55–67. 10.1080/00401706.1970.10488634
    1. Hong SW, Tong F (2017) Neural representation of form-contingent color filling-in in the early visual cortex. J Vis 17(13):10, 1–10. 10.1167/17.13.10
    1. Johnston A, Wright MJ (1985) Lower thresholds of motion for gratings as a function of eccentricity and contrast. Vision Res 25:179–185. 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90111-7
    1. Jones HE, Andolina IM, Shipp SD, Adams DL, Cudeiro J, Salt TE, Sillito AM (2015) Figure-ground modulation in awake primate thalamus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:7085–7090. 10.1073/pnas.1405162112
    1. Kastner S, De Weerd P, Ungerleider LG (2000) Texture segregation in the human visual cortex: a functional MRI study. J Neurophysiol 83:2453–2457. 10.1152/jn.2000.83.4.2453
    1. Klink PC, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis M-A, Roelfsema PR (2017) Distinct feedforward and feedback effects of microstimulation in visual cortex reveal neural mechanisms of texture segregation. Neuron 95:209–220.e3. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.033
    1. Kok P, de Lange FP (2014) Shape perception simultaneously up- and downregulates neural activity in the primary visual cortex. Curr Biol 24:1531–1535. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.042
    1. Kourtzi Z, Tolias AS, Altmann CF, Augath M, Logothetis NK (2003) Integration of local features into global shapes. Neuron 37:333–346. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01174-1
    1. Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Ruff DA, Kiani R, Bodurka J, Esteky H, Tanaka K, Bandettini PA (2008) Matching categorical object representations in inferior temporal cortex of man and monkey. Neuron 60:1126–1141. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.043
    1. Kuai S-G, Li W, Yu C, Kourtzi Z (2017) Contour integration over time: psychophysical and fMRI evidence. Cereb Cortex 27:3042–3051. 10.1093/cercor/bhw147
    1. Kwon O-S, Tadin D, Knill DC (2015) Unifying account of visual motion and position perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:8142–8147. 10.1073/pnas.1500361112
    1. Lamme VA. (1995) The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 15:1605–1615.
    1. Lamme VA, Roelfsema PR (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci 23:571–579. 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01657-x
    1. Lamme VA, Super H, Spekreijse H (1998a) Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 8:529–535. 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80042-1
    1. Lamme VA, Zipser K, Spekreijse H (1998b) Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:3263–3268. 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263
    1. Landy MS, Bergen JR (1991) Texture segregation and orientation gradient. Vision Res 31:679–691. 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90009-t
    1. Landy MS, Kojima H (2001) Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges. J Opt Soc Am A 18:2307–2320. 10.1364/JOSAA.18.002307
    1. Leventhal AG, Schall JD (1983) Structural basis of orientation sensitivity of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Comp Neurol 220:465–475. 10.1002/cne.902200408
    1. Marcus DS, van Essen DC (2002) Scene segmentation and attention in primate cortical areas V1 and V2. J Neurophysiol 88:2648–2658. 10.1152/jn.00916.2001
    1. Mazer JA, Vinje WE, McDermott J, Schiller PH, Gallant JL (2002) Spatial frequency and orientation tuning dynamics in area V1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:1645–1650. 10.1073/pnas.022638499
    1. Meng M, Remus DA, Tong F (2005) Filling-in of visual phantoms in the human brain. Nat Neurosci 8:1248–1254. 10.1038/nn1518
    1. Miyawaki Y, Uchida H, Yamashita O, Sato M-A, Morito Y, Tanabe HC, Sadato N, Kamitani Y (2008) Visual image reconstruction from human brain activity using a combination of multiscale local image decoders. Neuron 60:915–929. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.004
    1. Murray SO, Kersten D, Olshausen BA, Schrater P, Woods DL (2002) Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:15164–15169. 10.1073/pnas.192579399
    1. Nelson JI, Frost BJ (1978) Orientation-selective inhibition from beyond the classic visual receptive field. Brain Res 139:359–365. 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90937-x
    1. Nothdurft HC. (1991) Texture segmentation and pop-out from orientation contrast. Vision Res 31:1073–1078. 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90211-m
    1. Nothdurft HC. (1993) The role of features in preattentive vision: comparison of orientation, motion and color cues. Vision Res 33:1937–1958. 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90020-w
    1. Papale P, Leo A, Cecchetti L, Handjaras G, Kay KN, Pietrini P, Ricciardi E (2018) Foreground-background segmentation revealed during natural image viewing. eNeuro 5:ENEURO.0075-18.2018–18 10.1523/ENEURO.0075-18.2018
    1. Poltoratski S, Ling S, McCormack D, Tong F (2017) Characterizing the effects of feature salience and top-down attention in the early visual system. J Neurophysiol 118:564–573. 10.1152/jn.00924.2016
    1. Poltoratski S, Maier A, Newton AT, Tong F (2019) Figure-ground modulation in the human lateral geniculate nucleus is distinguishable from top-down attention. Curr Biol 29:2051–2057.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.068
    1. Poort J, Raudies F, Wannig A, Lamme VAF, Neumann H, Roelfsema PR (2012) The role of attention in figure-ground segregation in areas V1 and V4 of the visual cortex. Neuron 75:143–156. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.032
    1. Poort J, Self MW, van Vugt B, Malkki H, Roelfsema PR (2016) Texture segregation causes early figure enhancement and later ground suppression in areas V1 and V4 of visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 26:3964–3976. 10.1093/cercor/bhw235
    1. Qiu FT, Sugihara T, von der Heydt R (2007) Figure-ground mechanisms provide structure for selective attention. Nat Neurosci 10:1492–1499. 10.1038/nn1989
    1. Roelfsema PR. (2006) Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annu Rev Neurosci 29:203–227. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112939
    1. Rossi A, Desimone R, Ungerleider L (2001) Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex of macaques. J Neurosci 21:1698–1709. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01698.2001
    1. Sasaki Y, Watanabe T (2004) The primary visual cortex fills in color. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:18251–18256. 10.1073/pnas.0406293102
    1. Schira MM, Fahle M, Donner TH, Kraft A, Brandt SA (2004) Differential contribution of early visual areas to the perceptual process of contour processing. J Neurophysiol 91:1716–1721. 10.1152/jn.00380.2003
    1. Scholte HS, Jolij J, Fahrenfort JJ, Lamme VAF (2008) Feedforward and recurrent processing in scene segmentation: electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Cogn Neurosci 20:2097–2109. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20142
    1. Self MW, Kooijmans RN, Supèr H, Lamme VA, Roelfsema PR (2012) Different glutamate receptors convey feedforward and recurrent processing in macaque V1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:11031–11036. 10.1073/pnas.1119527109
    1. Self MW, van Kerkoerle T, Supèr H, Roelfsema PR (2013) Distinct roles of the cortical layers of area V1 in figure-ground segregation. Curr Biol 23:2121–2129. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.013
    1. Self MW, Peters JC, Possel JK, Reithler J, Goebel R, Ris P, Jeurissen D, Reddy L, Claus S, Baayen JC, Roelfsema PR (2016) The effects of context and attention on spiking activity in human early visual cortex. PLoS Biol 14:e1002420. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002420
    1. Self MW, Jeurissen D, van Ham AF, van Vugt B, Poort J, Roelfsema PR (2019) The segmentation of proto-objects in the monkey primary visual cortex. Curr Biol 29:1019–1029.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.016
    1. Shushruth S, Mangapathy P, Ichida JM, Bressloff PC, Schwabe L, Angelucci A (2012) Strong recurrent networks compute the orientation tuning of surround modulation in the primate primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 32:308–321. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-11.2012
    1. Sillito AM, Grieve KL, Jones HE, Cudeiro J, Davis J (1995) Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities. Nature 378:492–496. 10.1038/378492a0
    1. Singh KD, Smith AT, Greenlee MW (2000) Spatiotemporal frequency and direction sensitivities of human visual areas measured using fMRI. Neuroimage 12:550–564. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0642
    1. Somers DC, Dale AM, Seiffert AE, Tootell RB (1999) Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:1663–1668. 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1663
    1. Supèr H, Spekreijse H, Lamme VA (2001) Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nat Neurosci 4:304–310. 10.1038/85170
    1. Swisher JD, Sexton JA, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Tong F (2012) Multishot versus single-shot pulse sequences in very high field fMRI: a comparison using retinotopic mapping. PLoS One 7:e34626. 10.1371/journal.pone.0034626
    1. Thielscher A, Kölle M, Neumann H, Spitzer M, Grön G (2008) Texture segmentation in human perception: a combined modeling and fMRI study. Neuroscience 151:730–736. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.11.040
    1. Thirion B, Duchesnay E, Hubbard E, Dubois J, Poline J-B, Lebihan D, Dehaene S (2006) Inverse retinotopy: inferring the visual content of images from brain activation patterns. Neuroimage 33:1104–1116. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.062
    1. Tootell RB, Hadjikhani N, Hall EK, Marrett S, Vanduffel W, Vaughan JT, Dale AM (1998) The retinotopy of visual spatial attention. Neuron 21:1409–1422. 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80659-5
    1. Treisman AM, Gelade G (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12:97–136. 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
    1. Vinberg J, Grill-Spector K (2008) Representation of shapes, edges, and surfaces across multiple cues in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 99:1380–1393. 10.1152/jn.01223.2007
    1. Wandell BA, Winawer J (2015) Computational neuroimaging and population receptive fields. Trends Cogn Sci 19:349–357. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.009
    1. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56:366–383. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012
    1. Wang W, Jones HE, Andolina IM, Salt TE, Sillito AM (2006) Functional alignment of feedback effects from visual cortex to thalamus. Nat Neurosci 9:1330–1336. 10.1038/nn1768
    1. Xu X, Ichida J, Shostak Y, Bonds AB, Casagrande VA (2002) Are primate lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) cells really sensitive to orientation or direction? Vis Neurosci 19:97–108. 10.1017/s0952523802191097
    1. Zhang X, Zhaoping L, Zhou T, Fang F (2012) Neural activities in V1 create a bottom-up saliency map. Neuron 73:183–192. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.035
    1. Zhaoping L. (2003) V1 mechanisms and some figure–ground and border effects. J Physiol Paris 97:503–515. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.01.008
    1. Zipser K, Lamme VA, Schiller PH (1996) Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 16:7376–7389.

Source: PubMed

3
Sottoscrivi