Prefrontal Gamma Oscillations Encode Tonic Pain in Humans

Enrico Schulz, Elisabeth S May, Martina Postorino, Laura Tiemann, Moritz M Nickel, Viktor Witkovsky, Paul Schmidt, Joachim Gross, Markus Ploner, Enrico Schulz, Elisabeth S May, Martina Postorino, Laura Tiemann, Moritz M Nickel, Viktor Witkovsky, Paul Schmidt, Joachim Gross, Markus Ploner

Abstract

Under physiological conditions, momentary pain serves vital protective functions. Ongoing pain in chronic pain states, on the other hand, is a pathological condition that causes widespread suffering and whose treatment remains unsatisfactory. The brain mechanisms of ongoing pain are largely unknown. In this study, we applied tonic painful heat stimuli of varying degree to healthy human subjects, obtained continuous pain ratings, and recorded electroencephalograms to relate ongoing pain to brain activity. Our results reveal that the subjective perception of tonic pain is selectively encoded by gamma oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex. We further observed that the encoding of subjective pain intensity experienced by the participants differs fundamentally from that of objective stimulus intensity and from that of brief pain stimuli. These observations point to a role for gamma oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex in ongoing, tonic pain and thereby extend current concepts of the brain mechanisms of pain to the clinically relevant state of ongoing pain. Furthermore, our approach might help to identify a brain marker of ongoing pain, which may prove useful for the diagnosis and therapy of chronic pain.

Keywords: electroencephalography; gamma oscillations; ongoing pain; pain perception; prefrontal cortex.

© The Author 2015. Published by Oxford University Press.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Time courses of pain intensity and stimulus intensity. Group mean time courses of subjective pain intensity and objective stimulus intensity during tonic painful heat stimulation of the left hand. Pain intensity was continuously rated on a VAS anchored at no pain and worst tolerable pain. Shaded areas around the curves depict the standard error of the mean. The light gray section indicates the time window used for the analysis. For display purposes, mean time courses were low-pass filtered at 0.1 Hz.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Neurophysiological encoding of pain intensity and stimulus intensity during tonic pain. (A) Topographies of the relationship between pain intensity/stimulus intensity and brain activity as assessed by LMMs. LMMs were calculated for theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (14–29 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz) frequencies. Positive and negative relationships are depicted by warm and cold colors, respectively. Electrodes with a significant relationship between pain/stimulus intensity and brain activity after FDR correction for multiple testing are marked by bold black dots. (B) Frequency spectra of the relationship between pain/stimulus intensity and brain activity. LMMs were calculated for frequencies between 1 and 100 Hz for electrodes which had shown a significant relationship between pain/stimulus intensity and brain activity as displayed in (A). The strongest (positive) relationship between pain intensity and brain activity was observed at 84 Hz, and the strongest (negative) relationship between stimulus intensity and brain activity was found at 15 Hz.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Control conditions. Topographies of the relationship between brain activity and bar length rating in the visual control condition (upper row) and between brain activity and the inverted time course of pain intensity in the tonic pain condition (lower row) as assessed by LMMs. LMMs were calculated for theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (14–29 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz) frequencies. Positive and negative relationships are depicted by warm and cold colors, respectively. No significant relationships were observed after FDR correction for multiple testing.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Neurophysiological encoding of pain intensity and stimulus intensity during phasic pain. (A) Group mean time–frequency representation of neuronal responses to phasic painful stimuli at electrode FCz. Neuronal responses are displayed as percent signal change relative to a pre-stimulus baseline (−1000 to 0 ms). Positive and negative signal changes are depicted by warm and cold colors, respectively. (B) Topographies of the relationship between stimulus/pain intensity and brain activity as assessed by LMMs. LMMs were calculated for time–frequency windows defined from previous studies (theta, 4–8 Hz, 0.15–0.35 s; alpha, 9–13 Hz, 0.47–0.70 s; beta, 14–29 Hz, 0.30–0.50 s; and gamma, 76–86 Hz, 0.20–0.30 s). Positive and negative relationships are depicted by warm and cold colors, respectively. Electrodes with a significant relationship between pain/stimulus intensity and brain activity after FDR correction for multiple testing are marked by bold black dots.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Brain sources encoding tonic pain. Locations of (A) the strongest relationship between subjective pain intensity and brain activity in the gamma band (30–100 Hz) and (B) the strongest relationship between objective stimulus intensity and brain activity in the beta band (14–29 Hz) as assessed by LMM in source space. Positive and negative relationships are depicted by warm and cold colors, respectively. MNI coordinates of strongest relationships (peak locations) were −4, 34, 36 in (A) and 8, −16, 68 in (B).

References

    1. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Schweinhardt P. 2013. Representation of pain in the brain. In: McMahon SB, KoltzenburgTracey MI, Turk DC, editors. Wall and Melzack's Textbook of pain. 6th ed Philadelphia: Elsevier; p. 111–128.
    1. Baliki MN, Baria AT, Apkarian AV. 2011. The cortical rhythms of chronic back pain. J Neurosci. 31:13981–13990.
    1. Baliki MN, Chialvo DR, Geha PY, Levy RM, Harden RN, Parrish TB, Apkarian AV. 2006. Chronic pain and the emotional brain: specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. J Neurosci. 26:12165–12173.
    1. Barr MS, Farzan F, Davis KD, Fitzgerald PB, Daskalakis ZJ. 2013. Measuring GABAergic inhibitory activity with TMS-EEG and its potential clinical application for chronic pain. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 8:535–546.
    1. Borsook D, Edwards R, Elman I, Becerra L, Levine J. 2013. Pain and analgesia: the value of salience circuits. Prog Neurobiol. 104:93–105.
    1. Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D. 2006. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain. 10:287–333.
    1. Buzsaki G, Wang XJ. 2012. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 35:203–225.
    1. Chang PF, Arendt-Nielsen L, Chen AC. 2002. Dynamic changes and spatial correlation of EEG activities during cold pressor test in man. Brain Res Bull. 57:667–675.
    1. Chen AC, Rappelsberger P. 1994. Brain and human pain: topographic EEG amplitude and coherence mapping. Brain Topogr. 7:129–140.
    1. Davis KD, Racine E, Collett B. 2012. Neuroethical issues related to the use of brain imaging: can we and should we use brain imaging as a biomarker to diagnose chronic pain? Pain. 153:1555–1559.
    1. Derbyshire SW, Jones AK. 1998. Cerebral responses to a continual tonic pain stimulus measured using positron emission tomography. Pain. 76:127–135.
    1. Di Piero V, Ferracuti S, Sabatini U, Pantano P, Cruccu G, Lenzi GL. 1994. A cerebral blood flow study on tonic pain activation in man. Pain. 56:167–173.
    1. Donner TH, Siegel M. 2011. A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends Cogn Sci. 15:191–199.
    1. Dowman R, Rissacher D, Schuckers S. 2008. EEG indices of tonic pain-related activity in the somatosensory cortices. Clin Neurophysiol. 119:1201–1212.
    1. Ferracuti S, Seri S, Mattia D, Cruccu G. 1994. Quantitative EEG modifications during the Cold Water Pressor Test: hemispheric and hand differences. Int J Psychophysiol. 17:261–268.
    1. Foss JM, Apkarian AV, Chialvo DR. 2006. Dynamics of pain: fractal dimension of temporal variability of spontaneous pain differentiates between pain states. J Neurophysiol. 95:730–736.
    1. Garcia-Larrea L, Frot M, Valeriani M. 2003. Brain generators of laser-evoked potentials: from dipoles to functional significance. Neurophysiol Clin. 33:279–292.
    1. Garcia-Larrea L, Peyron R. 2013. Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: a review. Pain. 154(Suppl 1):S29–S43.
    1. Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T. 2002. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage. 15:870–878.
    1. Goncharova II, McFarland DJ, Vaughan TM, Wolpaw JR. 2003. EMG contamination of EEG: spectral and topographical characteristics. Clin Neurophysiol. 114:1580–1593.
    1. Gross J, Kujala J, Hamalainen M, Timmermann L, Schnitzler A, Salmelin R. 2001. Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 98:694–699.
    1. Gross J, Schnitzler A, Timmermann L, Ploner M. 2007. Gamma oscillations in human primary somatosensory cortex reflect pain perception. PLoS Biol. 5:e133.
    1. Hashmi JA, Baliki MN, Huang L, Baria AT, Torbey S, Hermann KM, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV. 2013. Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain. 136:2751–2768.
    1. Hauck M, Lorenz J, Engel AK. 2007. Attention to painful stimulation enhances gamma-band activity and synchronization in human sensorimotor cortex. J Neurosci. 27:9270–9277.
    1. Hipp JF, Siegel M. 2013. Dissociating neuronal gamma-band activity from cranial and ocular muscle activity in EEG. Front Hum Neurosci. 7:338.
    1. Hsieh JC, Belfrage M, Stone-Elander S, Hansson P, Ingvar M. 1995. Central representation of chronic ongoing neuropathic pain studied by positron emission tomography. Pain. 63:225–236.
    1. Hu L, Peng W, Valentini E, Zhang Z, Hu Y. 2013. Functional features of nociceptive-induced suppression of alpha band electroencephalographic oscillations. J Pain. 14:89–99.
    1. Jensen MP, Day MA, Miro J. 2014. Neuromodulatory treatments for chronic pain: efficacy and mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurol. 10:167–178.
    1. Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, Sejnowski TJ. 2000. Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology. 37:163–178.
    1. Kupers R, Danielsen ER, Kehlet H, Christensen R, Thomsen C. 2009. Painful tonic heat stimulation induces GABA accumulation in the prefrontal cortex in man. Pain. 142:89–93.
    1. Legrain V, Iannetti GD, Plaghki L, Mouraux A. 2011. The pain matrix reloaded: a salience detection system for the body. Prog Neurobiol. 93:111–124.
    1. May ES, Butz M, Kahlbrock N, Hoogenboom N, Brenner M, Schnitzler A. 2012. Pre- and post-stimulus alpha activity shows differential modulation with spatial attention during the processing of pain. Neuroimage. 62:1965–1974.
    1. Mouraux A, Guerit JM, Plaghki L. 2003. Non-phase locked electroencephalogram (EEG) responses to CO2 laser skin stimulations may reflect central interactions between A-delta- and C-fibre afferent volleys. Clin Neurophysiol. 114:710–722.
    1. Nir RR, Sinai A, Moont R, Harari E, Yarnitsky D. 2012. Tonic pain and continuous EEG: prediction of subjective pain perception by alpha-1 power during stimulation and at rest. Clin Neurophysiol. 123:605–612.
    1. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. 2011. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011:156869.
    1. Owen DG, Clarke CF, Ganapathy S, Prato FS, St Lawrence KS. 2010. Using perfusion MRI to measure the dynamic changes in neural activation associated with tonic muscular pain. Pain. 148:375–386.
    1. Palva S, Palva JM. 2011. Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front Psychol. 2:204.
    1. Peng W, Hu L, Zhang Z, Hu Y. 2014. Changes of spontaneous oscillatory activity to tonic heat pain. PLoS ONE. 9:e91052.
    1. Ploner M, Gross J, Timmermann L, Pollok B, Schnitzler A. 2006a. Oscillatory activity reflects the excitability of the human somatosensory system. Neuroimage. 32:1231–1236.
    1. Ploner M, Gross J, Timmermann L, Pollok B, Schnitzler A. 2006b. Pain suppresses spontaneous brain rhythms. Cereb Cortex. 16:537–540.
    1. Romo R, de Lafuente V. 2013. Conversion of sensory signals into perceptual decisions. Prog Neurobiol. 103:41–75.
    1. Rossiter HE, Worthen SF, Witton C, Hall SD, Furlong PL. 2013. Gamma oscillatory amplitude encodes stimulus intensity in primary somatosensory cortex. Front Hum Neurosci. 7:362.
    1. Schreckenberger M, Siessmeier T, Viertmann A, Landvogt C, Buchholz HG, Rolke R, Treede RD, Bartenstein P, Birklein F. 2005. The unpleasantness of tonic pain is encoded by the insular cortex. Neurology. 64:1175–1183.
    1. Schulz E, Tiemann L, Schuster T, Gross J, Ploner M. 2011. Neurophysiological coding of traits and states in the perception of pain. Cereb Cortex. 21:2408–2414.
    1. Shackman AJ, Salomons TV, Slagter HA, Fox AS, Winter JJ, Davidson RJ. 2011. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 12:154–167.
    1. Shao S, Shen K, Yu K, Wilder-Smith EP, Li X. 2012. Frequency-domain EEG source analysis for acute tonic cold pain perception. Clin Neurophysiol. 123:2042–2049.
    1. Tiemann L, Schulz E, Gross J, Ploner M. 2010. Gamma oscillations as a neuronal correlate of the attentional effects of pain. Pain. 150:302–308.
    1. Tracey I, Mantyh PW. 2007. The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron. 55:377–391.
    1. Veerasarn P, Stohler CS. 1992. The effect of experimental muscle pain on the background electrical brain activity. Pain. 49:349–360.
    1. Wasan AD, Loggia ML, Chen LQ, Napadow V, Kong J, Gollub RL. 2011. Neural correlates of chronic low back pain measured by arterial spin labeling. Anesthesiology. 115:364–374.
    1. Zhang ZG, Hu L, Hung YS, Mouraux A, Iannetti GD. 2012. Gamma-band oscillations in the primary somatosensory cortex—a direct and obligatory correlate of subjective pain intensity. J Neurosci. 32:7429–7438.

Source: PubMed

3
Se inscrever