The effects of yoga and quiet rest on subjective levels of anxiety and physiological correlates: a 2-way crossover randomized trial

Kembra Albracht-Schulte, Jacalyn Robert-McComb, Kembra Albracht-Schulte, Jacalyn Robert-McComb

Abstract

Background: Rest or acute exercise can decrease state anxiety, with some evidence showing exercise to prevent laboratory-induced elevations in anxiety. No study has examined whether yoga provides short-term protection against laboratory-induced anxiety. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of an acute YogaFit session on state anxiety and measures of heart rate variability (HRV) to determine whether yoga provides short-term protection against emotional picture stimuli.

Methods: A randomized repeated-measures crossover clinical trial was performed. Forty healthy, female college students completed a 30 min session of YogaFit and a time-matched seated rest condition on separate days. After each condition, participants viewed 30 min of emotional picture stimuli. State anxiety, heart rate and time-domain and frequency-domain measures of HRV were assessed baseline, post- condition, and post-exposure to emotional stimuli. Data were analysed using a condition x time (2 × 3) repeated-measures ANOVA.

Results: Post-hoc comparisons indicate the following: (1) state anxiety significantly decreased from baseline to post-condition for both yoga and rest (p = 0.001) but returned to baseline values following exposure to emotional stimuli (p < 0.001) for both conditions; (2) heart rate decreased post-condition to post-exposure (p = 0.020) and baseline to post-exposure (p = 0.033) for both conditions; (3) time-domain measure of HRV showed a significant increase in HRV between baseline and post-condition (p = 0 .019), post-condition and post-exposure (p = 0 .007), and between baseline and post-exposure (p < 0.001).

Conclusions: Both YogaFit and seated rest were effective at acutely reducing state anxiety post-condition, but not at preventing an induced anxiety response post-exposure. Following exposure to the emotionally stimulating pictures, there was a shift from the high frequency-domain to the low frequency-domain and an increase in the time-domain measure of HRV for both the YogaFit and the quiet rest condition.

Trial registration: Retrospectively registered 2/16/2018, clinicaltrials.gov, Identifier: NCT03458702 .

Keywords: Affect; Autonomic function; Emotion; Heart rate variability; Quiet rest.

Conflict of interest statement

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments and with the Texas Tech University Human Protection Program. All procedures performed in this study were approved through the Institutional Review Board processes at Texas Tech University and all participants signed the consent form approved by the Institutional Review Board before pre-screening.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
The Consort Flow Diagram
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
(i) State anxiety scores at baseline, post-condition, and post-exposure for the yoga and rest conditions. No significant difference between the yoga and rest conditions. (ii) Combined state anxiety scores at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure. Significant decrease, **p = 0.001, between baseline and post-condition and significant increase, ***p < 0 .001, between post-condition and post-exposure. (iii) Heart rate (HR) at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure for the yoga and rest conditions. Significant interaction between condition & time, ***p < 0 .001. (iv) Combined HR for both conditions at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure. Significant decrease between post-condition and post-exposure, *p = 0 .020 and between baseline and post-exposure, * p = 0 .033. Note symbols or abbreviations on graphs: N.S.- not significant, *p < 0.05, **p = 0 .001, and ***p < 0 .001. Error bars = SEM
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
(i) Root Mean Square of Successive Differences in RR Intervals (RMSSD) at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure for the yoga and rest conditions. Significant condition x time interaction, * p = 0 .042. (ii) Combined RMSSD at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure. Significant increase between baseline and post-condition (p = 0 .019), post-condition and post-exposure (*p = 0 .007), and between baseline and post-exposure,*** p < 0 .001. (iii) Low-frequency power in normalized units (LFN) at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure for the yoga and rest conditions. No significant difference between the yoga and rest conditions. (iv) Combined LFN at baseline, post-condition and post-exposure. Significant increase,* p = 0 .008, between post-condition and post-exposure. Note symbols or abbreviations on graphs: N.S.- not significant, *p < 0 .05, **p = 0 .001, and ***p < 0 .001. Error bars = SEM

References

    1. Shinba T, Kariya N, Matsui Y, Ozawa N, Matsuda Y, K-i Y. Decrease in heart rate variability response to task is related to anxiety and depressiveness in normal subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;62(5):603–609. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01855.x.
    1. Lehrer P, Karavidas MK, Lu S, Coyle SM, Oikawa LO, Macor M, Calvano SE, Lowry SF. Voluntarily produced increases in heart rate variability modulate autonomic effects of endotoxin induced systemic inflammation: an exploratory study. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2010;35(4):303–315. doi: 10.1007/s10484-010-9139-5.
    1. Lehrer P, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Lu SE, Eckberg DL, Edelberg R, Shih WJ, Lin Y, Kuusela TA, Tahvanainen KU, et al. Heart rate variability biofeedback increases baroreflex gain and peak expiratory flow. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(5):796–805. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000089200.81962.19.
    1. Mini Alessio, Palomba Daniela, Angrilli Alessandro, Bravi Stefano. Emotional Information Processing and Visual Evoked Brain Potentials. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1996;83(1):143–152. doi: 10.2466/pms.1996.83.1.143.
    1. Bradley MaL, P.: The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In: The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment edn. Edited by Bradley MaL, P: Oxford University Press Series in Affective Science; 2007: 29–46.
    1. Spielberger C, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.; 1983.
    1. Kreibig SD. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(3):394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.
    1. Carmody J, Baer RA, LB Lykins E, Olendzki N. An empirical study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. J Clin Psychol. 2009;65(6):613–626. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20579.
    1. Chu IH, Lin YJ, Wu WL, Chang YK, Lin IM. Effects of yoga on heart rate variability and mood in women: a randomized controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med. 2015;21(12):789–795. doi: 10.1089/acm.2015.0138.
    1. Chu IH, Wu WL, Lin IM, Chang YK, Lin YJ, Yang PC. Effects of yoga on heart rate variability and depressive symptoms in women: a randomized controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med. 2017;23(4):310–316. doi: 10.1089/acm.2016.0135.
    1. Satyapriya M, Nagendra HR, Nagarathna R, Padmalatha V. Effect of integrated yoga on stress and heart rate variability in pregnant women. Int J Gynecol Obstet. 2009;104(3):218–222. doi: 10.1016/j.ijgo.2008.11.013.
    1. Shaw B. Beth Shaw's YogaFit. 2. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2009.
    1. Bahrke MS, Morgan WP. Anxiety reduction following exercise and meditation. Cogn Ther Res. 1978;2(4):323–333. doi: 10.1007/BF01172650.
    1. Smith JC, O'Connor PJ, Crabbe JB, Dishman RK. Emotional responsiveness after low- and moderate-intensity exercise and seated rest. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34(7):1158–1167. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200207000-00017.
    1. Cox RH, Thomas TR, Hinton PS, Donahue OM. Effects of acute 60 and 80% VO 2 max bouts of aerobic exercise on state anxiety of women of different age groups across time. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2004;75(2):165–175. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2004.10609148.
    1. Raglin JS, Morgan WP. Influence of exercise and quiet rest on state anxiety and blood pressure. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1987;19(5):456–463. doi: 10.1249/00005768-198710000-00006.
    1. Oliver JM, Simmons ME. Depression as measured by the DSM-III and the Beck depression inventory in an unselected adult population. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1984;52(5):892–898. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.5.892.
    1. Smith JC. Effects of emotional exposure on state anxiety after acute exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45(2):372–378. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31826d5ce5.
    1. Leicht AS, Allen GD, Hoey AJ. Influence of age and moderate-intensity exercise training on heart rate variability in young and mature adults. Can J Appl Physiol. 2003;28(3):446–461. doi: 10.1139/h03-033.
    1. Bai X, Li J, Zhou L, Li X. Influence of the menstrual cycle on nonlinear properties of heart rate variability in young women. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2009;297(2):H765–H774. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.01283.2008.
    1. Barnes LLB, Harp D, Jung WS. Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Educ Psychol Meas. 2002;62(4):603–618. doi: 10.1177/0013164402062004005.
    1. Malliani A, Pagani M, Lombardi F, Cerutti S. Cardiovascular neural regulation explored in the frequency domain. Circulation. 1991;84(2):482–492. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.84.2.482.
    1. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Task force of the European Society of Cardiology and the north American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Eur Heart J. 1996;17(3):354–81. .
    1. Borg G. Borg's perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1998.
    1. Frijda N. The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986.
    1. Silva DGD, McComb, Robert P., Schiller, Anita R., Slechten, Aurelie: The Environmental Kuznets Curve in Small Geographies (Preliminary and Incomplete). Conference Proceedings 2018.
    1. Jackson AS, Blair SN, Mahar MT, Wier LT, Ross RM, Stuteville JE. Prediction of functional aerobic capacity without exercise testing. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1990;22(6):863–870. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199012000-00021.
    1. American College of Sports Medicine . ACSM's guidelines for exercise testing and prescription. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2014.
    1. Morgan WP. Physical activity and mental health. In: Eckert H, Montoye JJ, editors. Exercise and Health. Edn. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1984. pp. 132–145.
    1. Verkuil B, Brosschot JF, Thayer JF. Cardiac reactivity to and recovery from acute stress: temporal associations with implicit anxiety. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;92(2):85–91. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.03.002.
    1. Robert-McComb JJ, Chyu M-C, Tacón A, Norman R. The effects of tai chi on measures of stress and coping style. Focus Altern Complement Ther. 2015;20(2):89–96. doi: 10.1111/fct.12179.
    1. Sztajzel J. Heart rate variability: a noninvasive electrocardiographic method to measure the autonomic nervous system. Swiss Med Wkly. 2004;134(35–36):514–522.
    1. Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An Overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and Norms. Frontiers in Public Health. 2017;5:258.
    1. Martelli D, Silvani A, McAllen RM, May CN, Ramchandra R. The low frequency power of heart rate variability is neither a measure of cardiac sympathetic tone nor of baroreflex sensitivity. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2014;307(7):H1005–H1012. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00361.2014.
    1. McCraty R, Atkinson M, Tomasino D, Bradley RT. The coherent heart: heart-brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. Integral Review. 2009;5(2):10–115.
    1. Tiller WA, McCraty R, Atkinson M. Cardiac coherence: a new, noninvasive measure of autonomic nervous system order. Altern Ther Health Med. 1996;2(1):52–65.
    1. Millis RM, Austin RE, Hatcher MD, Bond V, Faruque MU, Goring KL, Hickey BM, DeMeersman RE. Association of body fat percentage and heart rate variability measures of sympathovagal balance. Life Sci. 2010;86(5–6):153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2009.11.018.
    1. Lavie N. Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010;19(3):143–148. doi: 10.1177/0963721410370295.
    1. Taylor A. Physical activity, anxiety and stress. In: Biddle S, Fox K, Boutcher S, editors. Physical Activity and Psychological Well-being. London: Routledge; 2000. pp. 10–45.
    1. Ntoumanis N, Edmunds J, Duda JL. Understanding the coping process from a self-determination theory perspective. Br J Health Psychol. 2009;14(2):249–260. doi: 10.1348/135910708X349352.
    1. Huang C-J, Webb HE, Zourdos MC, Acevedo EO. Cardiovascular reactivity, stress, and physical activity. Front Physiol. 2013;4:314.

Source: PubMed

3
Se inscrever