A four-day Western-style dietary intervention causes reductions in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory and interoceptive sensitivity

Tuki Attuquayefio, Richard J Stevenson, Megan J Oaten, Heather M Francis, Tuki Attuquayefio, Richard J Stevenson, Megan J Oaten, Heather M Francis

Abstract

In animals, a Western style diet-high in saturated fat and added sugar-causes impairments in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory (HDLM) and perception of internal bodily state (interoception). In humans, while there is correlational support for a link between Western-style diet, HDLM, and interoception, there is as yet no causal data. Here, healthy individuals were randomly assigned to consume either a breakfast high in saturated fat and added sugar (Experimental condition) or a healthier breakfast (Control condition), over four consecutive days. Tests of HDLM, interoception and biological measures were administered before and after breakfast on the days one and four, and participants completed food diaries before and during the study. At the end of the study, the Experimental condition showed significant reductions in HDLM and reduced interoceptive sensitivity to hunger and fullness, relative to the Control condition. The Experimental condition also showed a markedly different blood glucose and triglyceride responses to their breakfast, relative to Controls, with larger changes in blood glucose across breakfast being associated with greater reductions in HDLM. The Experimental condition compensated for their energy-dense breakfast by reducing carbohydrate intake, while saturated fat intake remained consistently higher than Controls. This is the first experimental study in humans to demonstrate that a Western-style diet impacts HDLM following a relatively short exposure-just as in animals. The link between diet-induced HDLM changes and blood glucose suggests one pathway by which diet impacts HDLM in humans.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. HVLT scores.
Fig 1. HVLT scores.
Mean (± standard error) HVLT retention score (%) on test days one and four for the Experimental and Control groups.
Fig 2. Interoception scores.
Fig 2. Interoception scores.
Mean (± standard error) kilojoules (kJ) required to shift hunger and fullness ratings on days one and four for each group.
Fig 3. Blood glucose data.
Fig 3. Blood glucose data.
(A)—Mean (± standard error) pre- and post-prandial blood glucose levels at the start and end of the study for each group; (B)—Scatterplot of the negative linear relationship between HVLT retention score (%) by day (Day 4 –Day 1) and blood glucose levels (mg/dL) by time (post-breakfast–pre-breakfast).
Fig 4. Triglycerides data.
Fig 4. Triglycerides data.
Mean (± standard error) pre- and post-prandial triglyceride levels at the start and end of the study for each group.
Fig 5. Food diary data.
Fig 5. Food diary data.
An illustration of the compensation of energy intake across weeks and between groups, following the breakfast manipulation. Overall energy intake is greater in Week 2 relative to Week 1, with no differences between groups (i.e., caloric compensation).

References

    1. Squire L. Memory and the hippocampus–a synthesis of findings with rats, monkeys and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992;99: 195–231.
    1. Beilharz JE, Maniam J, Morris MJ. Short exposure to a diet rich in both fat and sugar or sugar alone impairs place, but not object recognition memory in rats. Brain Behav Immun 2014;37: 134–141. 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.11.016
    1. Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Walls EK, Jarrard LE. Memory inhibition and energy regulation. Physiol Behav. 2005; 86: 731–746. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.004
    1. Hargrave S, Jones S, Davidson TL. The outward spiral: a vicious cycle model of obesity and cognitive dysfunction. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016;9: 40–46. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.12.001
    1. Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: Links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav. 2005;103: 59–68.
    1. Parent MB, Darling JN, Henderson YO. Remembering to eat: hippocampal regulation of meal onset. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2014;306: R701–R713. 10.1152/ajpregu.00496.2013
    1. Attuquayefio TN, Stevenson RJ, Boakes RA, Oaten MJ, Yeomans MR, Mahmut M, Francis HM. A high-fat high-sugar diet predicts poorer hippocampal-related memory and a reduced ability to suppress wanting under satiety. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016; Accepted.
    1. Brannigan M, Stevenson RJ, Francis HM. Thirst interoception and its relationship to a Western-style diet. Physiol Behav. 2015;139: 423–429 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.050
    1. Francis HM, Stevenson RJ. Higher reported saturated fat and refined sugar intake is associated with reduced hippocampal-dependent memory and sensitivity to interoceptive signals. Behav Neurosci. 2011;125: 943–955. 10.1037/a0025998
    1. Gibson EL, Barr S, Jeanes YM. (2013). Habitual fat intake predicts memory function in younger women. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7: 838–849. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00838
    1. Greenwood CE, Winocur G. Learning and memory impairment in rats fed a high saturated fat diet. Behav Neural Biol. 1990;53: 74–87.
    1. Greenwood CE, Winocur G. Cognitive impairment in rats fed high-fat diets: a specific effect of saturated fatty-acid intake. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110: 451–459.
    1. Granholm A, Bimonte-Nelson HA, Moore AB, Nelson ME, Freeman LR, Sambamurti K. Effects of a Saturated Fat and High Cholesterol Diet on Memory and Hippocampal Morphology in the Middle-Aged Rat. J Alzheimers Dis. 2008;14: 133–145.
    1. Morrison CD, Pistell PJ, Ingram DK, Johnson WD, Liu Y, Fernandez-Kim SO, et al. High fat diet increases hippocampal oxidative stress and cognitive impairment in aged mice: implications for decreased Nrf2 signaling. J Neurochem. 2010;114: 1581–1589. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06865.x
    1. Greenwood CE, Winocur G. Glucose Treatment Reduces Memory Deficits in Young Adult Rats Fed High-Fat Diets. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2001;75: 179–189. 10.1006/nlme.2000.3964
    1. Jurdak N, Lichtenstein AH, Kanarek RB. Diet-induced obesity and spatial cognition in young male rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2008;11: 48–54. 10.1179/147683008X301333
    1. Jurdak N, Kanarek RB. (2009). Sucrose-induced obesity impairs novel object recognition learning in young rats. Physiol Behav. 2009;96: 1–5. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.023
    1. Beilharz JE, Maniam J, Morris MJ. Short-term exposure to a diet high in fat and sugar, or liquid sugar, selectively impairs hippocampal-dependent memory, with differential impacts on inflammation. Behav Brain Res. 2016;306: 1–7. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.03.018
    1. Darling JN, Ross AP, Bartness TJ, Parent MB. Predicting the effects of a high energy diet on fatty liver and hippocampal-dependent memory in male rats. Obesity. 2013;5: 910–917.
    1. Francis H, Mirzaei M, Pardey M, Haynes P, Cornish J. Proteomic analysis of the dorsal and ventral hippocampus of rats maintained on a high fat and refined sugar diet. Proteomics. 2013;13: 3076–3091. 10.1002/pmic.201300124
    1. Goldbart AD, Rowa BW, Kheirandish-Gozala L, Chenga Y, Brittiana KR, Gozala D. High fat/refined carbohydrate diet enhances the susceptibility to spatial learning deficits in rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Brain Res. 2006;1090: 190–196. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.046
    1. Hoane MR, Swan AA, Heck SE. The effects of a high-fat sucrose diet on functional outcome following cortical contusion injury in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2011;223: 119–124. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.028
    1. Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Different patterns of memory impairments accompany short and longer-term maintenance on a high-energy diet. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2010;36: 313–319.
    1. Kanoski SE, Zhang Y, Zheng W, Davidson TL. The Effects of a High-Energy Diet on Hippocampal Function and Blood-Brain Barrier Integrity in the Rat. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;21: 207–219. 10.3233/JAD-2010-091414
    1. Messier C, Whately K, Liang J, Du L, Puissant D. The effects of a high-fat, high-fructose, and combination diet on learning, weight, and glucose regulation in C57BL/6 mice. Behav Brain Res. 2007; 178: 139–145. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.12.011
    1. Molteni R, Barnard RJ, Ying Z, Roberts CK, Gomez-Pinilla F. A high-fat, refined sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor, neuronal plasticity, and learning. Neurosci. 2002;112: 803–814.
    1. Tran DMD, Westbrook RF. Rats Fed a Diet Rich in Fats and Sugars Are Impaired in the Use of Spatial Geometry. Psychol Sci. 2015;26: 1947–1957. 10.1177/0956797615608240
    1. Baym CL, Naiman AK, Monti JM, Raine LB, Drollette ES, Moore RD, et al. (2014). Dietary lipids are differentially associated with hippocampal dependent relational memory in prepubescent children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99: 1026–1032. 10.3945/ajcn.113.079624
    1. Burrows T, Goldman S, Pursey K, Lim R. Is there an association between dietary intake and academic achievement: a systematic review. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2016;10: 1–24.
    1. Gardener SL, Rainey-Smith SR, Barnes MB, Sohrabi HR, Weinborn M, et al. Dietary patterns and cognitive decline in an Australian study of ageing. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;20: 860–866. 10.1038/mp.2014.79
    1. Granic A, Davies K, Adamson A, Kirkwood T, Hill TR, et al. Dietary Patterns High in Red Meat, Potato, Gravy, and Butter Are Associated with Poor Cognitive Functioning but Not with Rate of Cognitive Decline in Very Old Adults. J Nutri: Nutri Dis. 2016;146: 365–274.
    1. Sass KJ, Sass A, Westerveld M, Lencz T, Novelly RA, Kim JH, Spencer DD. Specificity in the correlation of verbal memory and hippocampal neuron loss: Dissociation of memory, language, and verbal intellectual ability. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14: 662–672. 10.1080/01688639208402854
    1. Bonner-Jackson A, Mahmoud S, Miller J, Banks SJ. Verbal and non-verbal memory and hippocampal volumes in a memory clinic population. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2015;7: 61–70. 10.1186/s13195-015-0147-9
    1. Maki PM, Cohen MH, Weber K, Little DM, Fornelli D, Rubin LH, et al. Impairments in memory and hippocampal function in HIV-positive vs HIV-negative women. Neurology. 2009;72: 1661–1668. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a55f65
    1. Kramer JH, Schuff N, Reed BR, Munga D, Du A, Rosen HJ, et al. Hippocampal volume and retention in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10: 639–643. 10.1017/S1355617704104050
    1. Travis SG, Huang Y, Fujiwara E, Radomski A, Olsen F, Carter R, et al. High field structural MRI reveals specific episodic memory correlates in the subfields of the hippocampus. Neuropsychologia. 2014;53: 233–245. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.016
    1. Attuquayefio TN, Stevenson RJ. A systematic review of longer-term dietary interventions on human cognitive function: Emerging patterns and future directions. Appetite. 2015;95: 554–570. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.08.023
    1. Lee J, Pase M, Pipingas A, Raubenheimer J, Thurgood M, Villalon L, et al. Switching to a 10-day Mediterranean-style diet improves mood and cardiovascular function in a controlled crossover study. Nutrition. 2015;31: 647–652. 10.1016/j.nut.2014.10.008
    1. Martínez-Lapiscina EH, Clavero P, Toledo E, Estruch R, et al. Mediterranean diet improves cognition: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomised trial. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84: 1318–25. 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304792
    1. McMillan L, Owen L, Kras M, Scholey A. Behavioural effects of a 10-day Mediterranean diet. Results from a pilot study evaluating mood and cognitive performance. Appetite. 2011;56: 143–147. 10.1016/j.appet.2010.11.149
    1. Bayer-Carter JL, Green PS, Montine TJ, VanFossen B, Baker LD, Watson S, et al. Diet Intervention and Cerebrospinal Fluid Biomarkers in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch Neurol. 2011;68: 743 10.1001/archneurol.2011.125
    1. Nilsson A, Tovar J, Johansson M, Radeborg K, Björck I. A diet based on multiple functional concepts improves cognitive performance in healthy subjects. Nutr Metab (Lond). 2013;10: 49.
    1. Holloway CJ, Cochlin LE, Emmanuel Y, Murray A, Codreanu I, Edwards LM, et al. A high-fat diet impairs cardiac high-energy phosphate metabolism and cognitive function in healthy human subjects. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93: 748–755. 10.3945/ajcn.110.002758
    1. Edwards LM, Murray AJ, Holloway CJ, Carter EE, Kemp GJ, Codreanu I. et al. Short-term consumption of a high-fat diet impairs whole-body efficiency and cognitive function in sedentary men. FASEB J. 2011;25: 1088–1096. 10.1096/fj.10-171983
    1. Simpson PM, Surmon DJ, Wesnes KA, Wilcock GK. The Cognitive Drug Research Computerized Assessment System for Demented Patients: A Validation Study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1991;6: 95–102.
    1. Hoyland A, Lawton CL, Dye L. Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: A systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008,32: 72–85. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.006
    1. Clifton PG, Vickers SP, Somerville EM. Little and often: Ingestive behavior patterns following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112: 502–511.
    1. Hebben N, Corkin S, Eichenbaum H, Shedlack K. Diminished ability to interpret and report internal states after bilateral medial temporal resection: case H.M. Behav Neurosci. 1985;99: 1031–1039.
    1. Rozin P, Dow S, Moscovitch M, Rajaram S. What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychol Sci. 1998;9: 392–396.
    1. Davidson TL, Chan K, Jarrard LE, Kanoski SE, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC. Contributions of the Hippocampus and Medial Prefrontal Cortex to Energy and Body Weight Regulation. Hippocampus. 2009;19: 235–252. 10.1002/hipo.20499
    1. Davidson TL, Kanoski SE, Chan K, Clegg DJ, Benoit SC, Jarrard LE. Hippocampal lesions impair retention of discriminative responding based on energy state cues. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124: 97–105. 10.1037/a0018402
    1. Sample CH, Martin AA, Jones S, Hargrave SL, Davidson TL. Western-style diet impairs stimulus control by food deprivation state cues: Implications for obesogenic environments. Appetite. 2015;93: 13–23. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.018
    1. Stranahan AM, Norman ED, Lee K, Cutler RG, Telljohann RS, Egan JM, Mattson MP. Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus. 2008;18: 1085–1088. 10.1002/hipo.20470
    1. Hsu TM, Konanur VR, Taing L, Usui R, Kayser BD, Goran M, Kanoski SE. Effects of Sucrose and High Fructose Corn Syrup Consumption on Spatial Memory Function and Hippocampal Neuroinflammation in Adolescent Rats. Hippocampus. 2015;25: 227–239. 10.1002/hipo.22368
    1. Pistell PJ, Morrison CD, Gupta S, Knight AG, Keller JN, Ingram DK, Bruce-Keller AJ. Cognitive impairment following high fat diet consumption is associated with brain inflammation. J Neuroimmunol. 2010;219: 25–32. 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2009.11.010
    1. White CL, Pistell PJ, Purpera MN, Gupta S, Fernandez-Kim SO, Hise TL, et al. Effects of high fat diet on Morris maze performance, oxidative stress, and inflammation in rats: contributions of maternal diet. Neurobiological Disorders. 2009;35: 3–13.
    1. Kanoski SE, Meisel RL, Mullins AJ, Davidson TL. The effects of energy-rich diets on discrimination reversal learning and on BDNF in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2007;182: 57–66. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.05.004
    1. Wu A, Molteni R, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. A saturated fat diet aggravates the outcome of traumatic brain injury on hippocampal plasticity and cognitive function by reducing brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Neuroscience. 2003;119: 365–375.
    1. Wu A, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. The interplay between oxidative stress and brain-derived neurotrophic factor modulates the outcome of a saturated fat diet on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19: 1699–1707. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03246.x
    1. Francis HM, Stevenson RJ. The longer-term impacts of Western diet on human cognition and the brain. Appetite. 2013;62: 119–128.
    1. Kaplan RJ, Greenwood CE, Winocur G, Wolever T. (2000). Cognitive performance is associated with glucose regulation in healthy elderly persons and can be enhanced with glucose and dietary carbohydrates. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;72: 825–836.
    1. Kalmijn S, Feskens EJM, Launer LJ, Stijnen T, Kromhout D. Glucose intolerance, hyperinsulinaemia and cognitive function in a general population of elderly men. Diabetologia. 1995;38: 1096–1102.
    1. Vanhanen M, Koivisto K, Kuusisto J, Mykkänen L, Helkala E, Hänninen T, et al. Cognitive function in an elderly population with persistent impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes Care. 1998;21: 398–402.
    1. Gold S, Dziobek I, Sweat V, Tirsi A, Rogers K, Bruehl H, et al. Hippocampal damage and memory impairments as possible early brain complications of type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007;50: 711–9. 10.1007/s00125-007-0602-7
    1. Greenwood CE, Kaplan RJ, Hebblethwaite S, Jenkins DJA. Carbohydrate-Induced Memory Impairment in Adults With Type 2 Diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2003;26: 1961–1966.
    1. Stewart R, Liolista D. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia. Diabetes and Medicine. 1999;16: 93–112.
    1. Francis HM, Stevenson R. Validity and test-retest reliability of a short dietary questionnaire to assess intake of saturated fat and free sugars: a preliminary study. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2013;26: 234–242. 10.1111/jhn.12008
    1. Karlsson J, Persson LO, Sjöström L, Sullivan M. Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. Int J Obes. 2000;24: 1715–1725.
    1. Papathanasiou G, Georgoudis G, Georgakopolous D, Katsouras C, Kalfakakou V, Evangelou A. Criterion-related validity of the short International Physical Activity Questionnaire against exercise capacity in young adults. European Journal of Preventative Cardiology. 2010;17: 380–386.
    1. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SLT, Walters EE, Zaslavsky AM. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32: 959–976.
    1. Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: Development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5: 125–142.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2009.
    1. Sullivan K. Alternate forms of prose passages for the assessment of auditory–verbal memory. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20: 745–753. 10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.006
    1. Schnabel R. (2012). Overcoming the Challenge of Re-assessing Logical Memory. Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;26: 102–115. 10.1080/13854046.2011.640642
    1. Nelson HE, Willison J. National Adult Reading Test Manual (2nd edn.). NFER-Nelson: Windsor. 1991.
    1. Crawford JR, Deary IJ, Starr J, Whalley LJ. The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: A retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval. Psychol Med. 2001;31: 451–458.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997.
    1. National Diet and Nutrition Survey. Medical Research Council. c2008. Available at: (accessed on 11 June 2013).
    1. Australian Food, Supplement and Nutrient Database (AUSNUT). Australian Capital Territory (ACT): Food Standards Australia New Zealand. c2014.
    1. USDA National Nutrient Database for Standard Reference, Release 27 (USDA SR27). Nutrient Data Laboratory, U.S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service. 2014.
    1. Baker KD, Reichelt AC. Impaired fear extinction retention and increased anxiety-like behaviours induced by limited daily access to a high-fat/high-sugar diet in male rats: Implications for diet-induced prefrontal cortex dysregulation. Neurobiol Learn Mem.2016,136: 127–138. 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.002
    1. Furlong TM, Jayaweera HK, Balleine BW, Corbit LH. Binge-Like Consumption of a Palatable Food Accelerates Habitual Control of Behavior and Is Dependent on Activation of the Dorsolateral Striatum. J Neursci. 2014,34: 5012–5022.
    1. Tooze JA, Subar AF, Thompson FE, Troiano R, Schatzkin A, Kipnis V. Psychosocial predictors of energy underreporting in a large doubly labeled water study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;79: 795–804.
    1. Farquhar JW, Frank A, Gross RC, Reaven GM. Glucose, insulin and triglyceride responses to high and low carbohydrate diets in man. J Clin Invest. 1966;45: 1648–1656. 10.1172/JCI105472
    1. Hanson AJ, Bayer JL, Baker LD, Cholerton B, VanFossen B, Trittschuh E, et al. Differential Effects of Meal Challenges on Cognition, Metabolism, and Biomarkers for Apolipoprotein E ε4 Carriers and Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;48: 205–218. 10.3233/JAD-150273
    1. Koopman KE, Caan MWA, Nederveen AJ, Pels A, Ackermans MT, Fliers E, et al. Hypercaloric Diets With Increased Meal Frequency, but Not Meal Size, Increase Intrahepatic Triglycerides: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Hepatology. 2014;60: 545–553. 10.1002/hep.27149
    1. Marin I, Kipnis J. Learning and memory… and the immune system. Learn Mem. 2013;20: 601–606. 10.1101/lm.028357.112
    1. Yirmiya R, Goshen I. Immune modulation of learning, memory, neural plasticity and neurogenesis. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25: 181–213. 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.10.015
    1. Boitard C, Cavaroc A, Sauvant J, Aubert A, Castanon N, Layé S, Ferreira G. Impairment of hippocampal-dependent memory induced by juvenile high-fat diet intake is associated with enhanced hippocampal inflammation in rats. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;40: 9–17. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.03.005
    1. Schulze MB, Hoffmann K, Manson JE, Willett WC, Meigs JB, Weikert C, et al. Dietary pattern, inflammation, and incidence of type 2 diabetes in women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82: 675–684.
    1. Sørensen LB, Raben A, Stender S, Astrup A. Effect of sucrose on inflammatory markers in overweight humans. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82: 421–427.
    1. Cai D. Neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration in overnutrition-induced diseases. Trends Endocrin Metab. 2012,24: 40–47.
    1. Warner-Schmidt J, Duman R. Jippocampal neurogenesis: Opposing effects of stress and antidepressant treatment. Hippocampus, 2006;16: 239–249. 10.1002/hipo.20156
    1. Santarell L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressant. Science, 2003;301: 805–809/ 10.1126/science.1083328
    1. Schoeller DA. Limitations in the assessment of dietary energy intake by self-report. Metabolism. 1995. 44: 18–22.
    1. Macdiarmid J, Blundell J. (1998). Assessing dietary intake: Who, what and why of under-reporting. Nutr. Res. Rev. 1998. 11: 231–253. 10.1079/NRR19980017
    1. Tooze JA, Subar AF, Thompson FE, Troiano R, Schatzkin A, Kipnis V. Psychosocial predictors of energy underreporting in a large doubly labeled water study. Am. Soc. Clin. Nutr. 2004. 79: 795–804.
    1. Braam LAJLM, Ocké MC, Bas Bueno-de-Mesquita H, Seidell JC. Determinants of Obesity-related Underreporting of Energy Intake. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1998, 147: 1081–1086.

Source: PubMed

3
Se inscrever