Bleak Present, Bright Future: Online Episodic Future Thinking, Scarcity, Delay Discounting, and Food Demand

Yan Yan Sze, Jeffrey S Stein, Warren K Bickel, Rocco A Paluch, Leonard H Epstein, Yan Yan Sze, Jeffrey S Stein, Warren K Bickel, Rocco A Paluch, Leonard H Epstein

Abstract

Obesity is associated with steep discounting of the future and increased food reinforcement. Episodic future thinking (EFT), a type of prospective thinking, has been observed to reduce delay discounting (DD) and improve dietary decision making. In contrast, negative income shock (i.e., abrupt transitions to poverty) has been shown to increase discounting and may worsen dietary decision-making. Scalability of EFT training and protective effects of EFT against simulated negative income shock on DD and demand for food were assessed. In two experiments we showed online-administered EFT reliably reduced DD. Furthermore, EFT reduced DD and demand for fast foods even when challenged by negative income shock. Our findings suggest EFT is a scalable intervention that has implications for improving public health by reducing discounting of the future and demand for high energy dense food.

Keywords: Episodic future thinking; delay discounting; online interventions; reinforcing value of food; scarcity.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean log k changes from baseline to post-training (mean ± SEM) for small, medium, large reward magnitudes and overall log k between episodic future thinking (EFT) and episodic recent thinking (ERT) condition. A reduction in log k indicates less discounting of the future. As indicated in the figure, EFT was significantly different from ERT in each analysis.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean area-under-the-curve (AUC) values (mean ± SEM) for main effects of cue-type (left panel) and narrative type (right panel). Greater AUC indicates less discounting of the future.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean indices of food reinforcement (mean ± SEM) for main effects of cue-type (elasticity, top left panel; intensity, top right panel; Omax, bottom left panel; Breakpoint, bottom right panel. Greater elasticity values indicate more sensitive to price.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean intensity of demand for fast foods (mean ± SEM) for main effects of income narrative.

References

    1. Adam TC, Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiology & Behavior. 2007;91:449–458. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011.
    1. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology. 2000;19:586–592. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586.
    1. Amlung M, Petker T, Jackson J, Balodis I, MacKillop J. Steep discounting of delayed monetary and food rewards in obesity: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2016;46:2423–2434. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000866.
    1. Amlung MT, Acker J, Stojek MK, Murphy JG, MacKillop J. Is talk “cheap”? An initial investigation of the equivalence of alcohol purchase task performance for hypothetical and actual rewards. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research. 2012;36:716–724. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01656.x.
    1. Atance CM, O’Neill DK. Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5:533–539. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0.
    1. Baker F, Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:382–392.
    1. Benoit RG, Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:6771–6779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6559-10.2011.
    1. Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS. Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: ’s Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis. 2012;20:351–368. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpr057.
    1. Bickel WK, George Wilson A, Franck CT, Terry Mueller E, Jarmolowicz DP, Koffarnus MN, Fede SJ. Using crowd sourcing to compare temporal, social temporal, and probability discounting among obese and non-obese individuals. Appetite. 2014;75:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.018.
    1. Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, MacKillop J, Epstein LH, Carr K, Mueller ET, Waltz TJ. APA addiction syndrome handbook. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2012. The behavioral economics of reinforcement pathologies: novel approaches to addictive disorders; pp. 333–363.
    1. Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Gatchalian KM. The behavioral economics and neuroeconomics of reinforcer pathologies: implications for etiology and treatment of addiction. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2011;13:406–415. doi: 10.1007/s11920-011-0215-1.
    1. Bickel WK, Moody L, Quisenberry AJ, Ramey CT, Sheffer CE. A competing neurobehavioral decision systems model of SES-related health and behavioral disparities. Preventive Medicine. 2014;68:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.06.032.
    1. Bickel WK, Pitcock JA, Yi R, Angtuaco EJ. Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8839–8846. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-08.2009.
    1. Bickel WK, Wilson AG, Chen C, Koffarnus MN, Franck CT. Stuck in time: negative income shock constricts the temporal window of valuation spanning the future and the past. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0163051. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163051.
    1. Boyer P. Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12:219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003.
    1. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, Chideya S, Marchi KS, Metzler M, Posner S. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA. 2005;294:2879–2888. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.22.2879.
    1. Bromberg U, Wiehler A, Peters J. Episodic future thinking is related to impulsive decision making in healthy adolescents. Child Development. 2015;86:1458–1468. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12390.
    1. Carr KA, Daniel TO, Lin H, Epstein LH. Reinforcement pathology and obesity. Current Drug Abuse Reviews. 2011;4:190–196.
    1. Carr KA, Lin H, Fletcher KD, Epstein LH. Food reinforcement, dietary disinhibition and weight gain in nonobese adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2014;22:254–259. doi: 10.1002/oby.20392.
    1. Couper MP, Alexander GL, Zhang NH, Little RJA, Maddy N, Nowak MA, … Johnson CC. Engagement and retention: measuring breadth and depth of participant use of an online intervention. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2010;12:41–55. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1430.
    1. Daniel TO, Epstein LH. The future is now: comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite. 2013;71:120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.07.010.
    1. Daniel TO, Said M, Stanton CM, Epstein LH. Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and energy intake in children. Eating Behaviors. 2015;18:20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.03.006.
    1. Daniel TO, Sawyer A, Dong Y, Bickel WK, Epstein LH. Remembering versus imagining: when does episodic retrospection and episodic prospection aid decision making? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.06.005.
    1. Daniel TO, Stanton CM, Epstein LH. The future is now: reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2339–2342. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488780.
    1. Dassen FC, Jansen A, Nederkoorn C, Houben K. Focus on the future: episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite. 2016;96:327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.032.
    1. Daugherty JR, Brase GL. Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48:202–207. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.007.
    1. de Wit H, Flory JD, Acheson A, McCloskey M, Manuck SB. IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42:111–121. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.026.
    1. Du WJ, Green L, Myerson J. Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Record. 2002;52:479–492.
    1. Epper T, Fehr-Duda H, Bruhin A. Viewing the future through a warped lens: why uncertainty generates hyperbolic discounting. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 2011;43:169–203. doi: 10.1007/s11166-011-9129-x.
    1. Epstein LH, Carr KA, Lin H, Fletcher KD. Food reinforcement, energy intake, and macronutrient choice. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2011;94:12–18. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.010314.
    1. Epstein LH, Dearing KK, Roba LG. A questionnaire approach to measuring the relative reinforcing efficacy of snack foods. Eating Behaviors. 2010;11:67–73.
    1. Epstein LH, Jankowiak N, Fletcher KD, Carr KA, Nederkoorn C, Raynor HA, Finkelstein E. Women who are motivated to eat and discount the future are more obese. Obesity. 2014;22:1394–1399. doi: 10.1002/oby.20661.
    1. Epstein LH, Jankowiak N, Lin H, Paluch R, Koffarnus MN, Bickel WK. No food for thought: moderating effects of delay discounting and future time perspective on the relation between income and food insecurity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2014;100:884–890. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.079772.
    1. Epstein LH, Wright SM, Paluch RA, Leddy J, Hawk LW, Jaroni JL, … Lerman C. Food hedonics and reinforcement as determinants of laboratory food intake in smokers. Physiology & Behavior. 2004;81:511–517.
    1. Graham Thomas J, Seiden A, Koffarnus MN, Bickel WK, Wing RR. Delayed reward discounting and grit in men and women with and without obesity. Obesity Science & Practice. 2015;1:131–135. doi: 10.1002/osp4.12.
    1. Green L, Myerson J, McFadden E. Rate of temporal discounting decreases with amount of reward. Memory & Cognition. 1997;25:715–723.
    1. Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Delton AW, Robertson TE. The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: a life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;100:1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/a0022403.
    1. Haushofer J, Fehr E. On the psychology of poverty. Science. 2014;344:862–867. doi: 10.1126/science.1232491.
    1. Haushofer J, Schunk D, Fehr E. Negative income shocks increase discount rates. University of Zurich Working Paper; 2013.
    1. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. The Guilford Press; 2013.
    1. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 2014;67:451–470. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12028.
    1. Hill WG, Weinert C. An evaluation of an online intervention to provide social support and health education. Computers, Informatics, Nursing. 2004;22:282–288.
    1. Hursh SR, Silberberg A. Economic demand and essential value. Psychological Review. 2008;115:186–198. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.186.
    1. Jacobs EA, Bickel WK. Modeling drug consumption in the clinic using simulation procedures: Demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1999;7:412–426. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.7.4.412.
    1. Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2002;77:129–146. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.77-129.
    1. Johnson MW, Bickel WK. An algorithm for identifying nonsystematic delay-discounting data. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2008;16:264–274. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.3.264.
    1. Kirby KN, Marakovic NN. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1996;3:100–104. doi: 10.3758/Bf03210748.
    1. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1999;128:78–87.
    1. Koffarnus MN, Franck CT, Stein JS, Bickel WK. A modified exponential behavioral economic demand model to better describe consumption data. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2015;23:504–512. doi: 10.1037/pha0000045.
    1. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL. The PHQ-9: a new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatric Annals. 2002;32:509–515.
    1. Lagorio CH, Madden GJ. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behavioural Processes. 2005;69:173–187. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.003.
    1. Larsen RJ, Prizmic Z. The regulation of emotional well-being: overcoming the hedonic treadmill. In: Eid M, Larsen RJ, editors. The Science of Subjective Well-being. New York: The Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 258–289.
    1. Lawrance EC. Poverty and the rate of time preference - evidence from panel data. Journal of Political Economy. 1991;99:54–77. doi: 10.1086/261740.
    1. Lin H, Carr KA, Fletcher KD, Epstein LH. Food reinforcement partially mediates the effect of socioeconomic status on body mass index. Obesity. 2013;21:1307–1312. doi: 10.1002/Oby.20158.
    1. Lin H, Epstein LH. Living in the moment: effects of time perspective and emotional valence of episodic thinking on delay discounting. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;128:12–19. doi: 10.1037/a0035705.
    1. MacKillop J, Murphy JG. A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;89:227–233. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.01.002.
    1. Madden GJ, Begotka AM, Raiff BR, Kastern LL. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2003;11:139–145.
    1. Minimum Wage - Wage and Hour Division (WHD) U.S. Department of Labor; 2016. Apr 1, Retrieved from .
    1. Mullainathan S, Shafir E. Scarcity: why having too little means so much. New York City: Henry Holt and Company; 2013.
    1. Murphy JG, MacKillop J. Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;14:219–227. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.2.219.
    1. Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2001;76:235–243. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235.
    1. O’Neill J, Daniel TO, Epstein LH. Episodic future thinking reduces eating in a food court. Eating Behaviors. 2016;20:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.10.002.
    1. Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis PG. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making. 2010;5:411–419.
    1. Rasmussen EB, Lawyer SR, Reilly W. Percent body fat is related to delay and probability discounting for food in humans. Behavioural Processes. 2010;83:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.09.001.
    1. Rollins BY, Dearing KK, Epstein LH. Delay discounting moderates the effect of food reinforcement on energy intake among non-obese women. Appetite. 2010;55:420–425. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.07.014.
    1. Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2007;362:773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087.
    1. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/Nrn2213.
    1. Schoenborn CA, Adams PF, Barnes PM. Body weight status of adults: United States, 1997–98. US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2002.
    1. Serdula MK, Mokdad AH, Williamson DF, Galuska DA, Mendlein JM, Heath GW. Prevalence of attempting weight loss and strategies for controlling weight. JAMA. 1999;282:1353–1358. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.14.1353.
    1. Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science. 2013;1:213–220. doi: 10.1177/2167702612469015.
    1. Smoyer-Tomic KE, Spence JC, Raine KD, Amrhein C, Cameron N, Yasenovskiy V, … Healy J. The association between neighborhood socioeconomic status and exposure to supermarkets and fast food outlets. Health Place. 2008;14:740–754. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.12.001.
    1. Snider SE, LaConte SM, Bickel WK. Episodic future thinking: expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research. 2016;40:1558–1566. doi: 10.1111/acer.13112.
    1. SPSS. SPSS for Windows Release 6. Chicago, IL: SPSS; 1993.
    1. Steiger JH. Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods. 2004;9:164–182. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164.
    1. Stein JS, Koffarnus MN, Snider SE, Quisenberry AJ, Bickel WK. Identification and management of nonsystematic purchase task data: toward best practice. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2015;23:377–386. doi: 10.1037/pha0000020.
    1. Strathman A, Gleicher F, Boninger DS, Edwards CS. The consideration of future consequences - weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66:742–752. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.742.
    1. Swan M. Crowdsourced health research studies: an important emerging complement to clinical trials in the public health research ecosystem. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2012;14:e46. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1988.
    1. Systat Software. Systat 11.0. Richmond, CA: SYSTAT Software, Inc; 2004.
    1. Sze YY, Daniel TO, Kilanowski CK, Collins RL, Epstein LH. Web-based and mobile delivery of an episodic future thinking intervention for overweight and obese families: a feasibility study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2015;3:e97. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.4603.
    1. Temple JL, Epstein LH. Sensitization of food reinforcement is related to weight status and baseline food reinforcement. International Journal of Obesity. 2012;36:1102–1107. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.210.
    1. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
    1. Weller RE, Cook EW, 3rd, Avsar KB, Cox JE. Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite. 2008;51:563–569. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.010.
    1. Wen X, Higgins ST, Xie C, Epstein LH. Improving public acceptability of using financial incentives for smoking cessation during pregnancy: a randomized controlled experiment. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2015 doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv204.
    1. Yong E. Replication studies: bad copy. Nature. 2012;485:298–300. doi: 10.1038/485298a.
    1. Yu J, Liu L, Collins RL, Vincent PC, Epstein LH. Analytical problems and suggestions in the analysis of behavioral economic demand curves. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2014;49:178–192. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2013.862491.

Source: PubMed

3
Se inscrever