P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Rik van Dinteren, Martijn Arns, Marijtje L A Jongsma, Roy P C Kessels, Rik van Dinteren, Martijn Arns, Marijtje L A Jongsma, Roy P C Kessels

Abstract

Background: The P300 component of the event-related potential is a large positive waveform that can be extracted from the ongoing electroencephalogram using a two-stimuli oddball paradigm, and has been associated with cognitive information processing (e.g. memory, attention, executive function). This paper reviews the development of the auditory P300 across the lifespan.

Methodology/principal findings: A systematic review and meta-analysis on the P300 was performed including 75 studies (n = 2,811). Scopus was searched for studies using healthy subjects and that reported means of P300 latency and amplitude measured at Pz and mean age. These findings were validated in an independent, existing cross-sectional dataset including 1,572 participants from ages 6-87. Curve-fitting procedures were applied to obtain a model of P300 development across the lifespan. In both studies logarithmic Gaussian models fitted the latency and amplitude data best. The P300 latency and amplitude follow a maturational path from childhood to adolescence, resulting in a period that marks a plateau, after which degenerative effects begin. We were able to determine ages that mark a maximum (in P300 amplitude) or trough (in P300 latency) segregating maturational from degenerative stages. We found these points of deflection occurred at different ages.

Conclusions/significance: It is hypothesized that latency and amplitude index different aspects of brain maturation. The P300 latency possibly indexes neural speed or brain efficiency. The P300 amplitude might index neural power or cognitive resources, which increase with maturation.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Schematic overview of the oddball…
Figure 1. Schematic overview of the oddball paradigm and an example of an ERP.
Figure 2. Flowchart depicting the number of…
Figure 2. Flowchart depicting the number of exclusions per exclusion rationale in the literature selection.
Figure 3. P300 latency and amplitude trajectories…
Figure 3. P300 latency and amplitude trajectories across the lifespan as obtained from the meta-analysis.
Dots represent (subgroups from a) study. Error bars represent SEM.
Figure 4. P300 latency and amplitude trajectories…
Figure 4. P300 latency and amplitude trajectories across the lifespan as obtained from the cross-sectional dataset.
Dots represent scores from individual participants.
Figure 5. Graphical summary of the found…
Figure 5. Graphical summary of the found trajectories in the cross-sectional dataset.
Dots represent the number of errors. Error bars represent SEM.
Figure 6. Trajectory of fractional anisotropy (FA)…
Figure 6. Trajectory of fractional anisotropy (FA) across the lifespan.
Adapted from Brickman et(2012). Data points were estimated using DigitizeIt 1.6.1 and curve-fitted using Graphpad Prism 6.0.

References

    1. Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER (1965) Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty. Science 150: 1187–1188.
    1. Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118: 2128–2148 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
    1. Ritter W, Vaughan HG (1969) Averaged evoked responses in vigilance and discrimination: a reassessment. Science 164: 326–328.
    1. Polich J (2004) Clinical application of the P300 event-related brain potential. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 15: 133–161.
    1. Polich J (1996) Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. Psychophysiology 33: 334–353.
    1. Patel SH, Azzam PN (2005) Characterization of N200 and P300: selected studies of the Event-Related Potential. Int J Med Sci 2: 147–154.
    1. Squires NK, Squires KC, Hillyard SA (1975) Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 38: 387–401.
    1. Snyder E, Hillyard SA (1976) Long-latency evoked potentials to irrelevant, deviant stimuli. Behav Biol 16: 319–331.
    1. Courchesne E, Kilman BA, Galambos R, Lincoln AJ (1984) Autism: processing of novel auditory information assessed by event-related brain potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 59: 238–248.
    1. Duncan-Johnson CC, Kopell BS (1981) The Stroop effect: brain potentials localize the source of interference. Science 214: 938–940.
    1. Verleger R (2010) Popper and P300: Can the view ever be falsified that P3 latency is a specific indicator of stimulus evaluation? Clin Neurophysiol 121: 1371–1372 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.01.038
    1. Verleger R (1997) On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. Psychophysiology 34: 131–156.
    1. Donchin E (1981) Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!…Surprise? Psychophysiology 18: 493–513.
    1. Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F (1999) Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
    1. Verleger R (1988) Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 343–356.
    1. Altenmüller EO, Gerloff G (1998) Psychophysiology and EEG, in: Niedermeyer E, Da Silva FHL (2004) Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
    1. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM (2001) Two- and three-stimuli auditory oddball ERP tasks and neuropsychological measures in aging. Neuroreport 12: 3149–3153.
    1. Shajahan PM, O’Carroll RE, Glabus MF, Ebmeier KP, Blackwood DH (1997) Correlation of auditory ‘oddball’ P300 with verbal memory deficits in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 27: 579–586.
    1. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB (2003) Effects of auditory stimulus intensity and hearing threshold on the relationship among P300, age, and cognitive function. Clin Neurophysiol 114: 799–807.
    1. Sachs G, Anderer P, Margreiter N, Semlitsch H, Saletu B, et al. (2004) P300 event-related potentials and cognitive function in social phobia. Psychiatry Res 131: 249–261 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.05.005
    1. Polich J, Kok A (1995) Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol Psychol 41: 103–146.
    1. Huster RJ, Westerhausen R, Herrmann CS (2011) Sex differences in cognitive control are associated with midcingulate and callosal morphology. Brain Struct Funct 215: 225–235 10.1007/s0042901002892
    1. Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM, Müller D, Leinsinger G, Juckel G, et al. (2001) The effect of the skull on event-related P300. Clin Neurophysiol 112: 1773–1776.
    1. Williams LM, Simms E, Clark CR, Paul RH, Rowe D, et al. (2005) The test-retest reliability of a standardized neurocognitive and neurophysiological test battery: “neuromarker”. Int J Neurosci 115: 1605–1630 10.1080/00207450590958475
    1. van Beijsterveldt CE, van Baal GC (2002) Twin and family studies of the human electroencephalogram: a review and a meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 61: 111–138.
    1. Polich J, Ladish C, Burns T (1990) Normal variation of P300 in children: age, memory span, and head size. Int J Psychophysiol 9: 237–248.
    1. Sangal B, Sangal M, Belisle C (1998) P300 Latency and Age: A Quadratic Regression Explains Their Relationship from Age 5 to 85. Clin EEG Neurosci 29: 1–6 10.1177/155005949802900105
    1. Rozhkov VP, Sergeeva EG, Soroko SI (2009) Age dynamics of evoked brain potentials in involuntary and voluntary attention to a deviant stimulus in schoolchildren from the northern region. Neurosci Behav Physiol 39: 851–863 10.1007/s110550099210-y
    1. Tsai M-L, Hung K-L, Tao-Hsin Tung W, Chiang T-R (2012) Age-changed normative auditory event-related potential value in children in Taiwan. J Formos Med Assoc 111: 245–252 10.1016/j.jfma.2011.01.009
    1. Walhovd KB, Rosquist H, Fjell AM (2008) P300 amplitude age reductions are not caused by latency jitter. Psychophysiology 45: 545–553 10.1111/j.14698986.2008.00661.x
    1. Rossini PM, Rossi S, Babiloni C, Polich J (2007) Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: from normal aging to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol 83: 375–400 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.07.010
    1. Ashford JW, Coburn KL, Rose TL, Bayley PJ (2011) P300 energy loss in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 26 Suppl 3229–238 10.3233/JAD-2011-0061
    1. Kuba M, Kremláček J, Langrová J, Kubová Z, Szanyi J, et al. (2012) Aging effect in pattern, motion and cognitive visual evoked potentials. Vision Res 62C: 9–16 10.1016/j.visres.2012.03.014
    1. Ehlers CL, Wall TL, Garcia-Andrade C, Phillips E (2001) Auditory P3 findings in mission Indian youth. J Stud Alcohol 62: 562–570.
    1. Beauchamp MS, Beurlot MR, Fava E, Nath AR, Parikh NA, et al. (2011) The developmental trajectory of brain-scalp distance from birth through childhood: implications for functional neuroimaging. PLoS One 6: e24981 10.1371/journal.pone.0024981
    1. Salthouse TA (2000) Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol Psychol 54: 35–54.
    1. Turner RM, Bird SM, Higgins JP (2013) The impact of study size on meta-analyses: examination of underpowered studies in Cochrane reviews. PLoS One 8: e59202 10.1371/journal.pone.0059202
    1. Kraemer HC, Gardner C, Brooks III JO, Yesavage JA (1998) Advantages of excluding underpowered studies in meta-analysis: Inclusionist versus exclusionist viewpoints. Psychological Methods 3: 23–31.
    1. Müller BW, Specka M, Steinchen N, Zerbin D, Lodemann E, et al. (2007) Auditory target processing in methadone substituted opiate addicts: the effect of nicotine in controls. BMC Psychiatry 7: 63 10.1186/1471-244X-7-63
    1. Paul RH, Gunstad J, Cooper N, Williams LM, Clark CR, et al. (2007) Cross-cultural assessment of neuropsychological performance and electrical brain function measures: additional validation of an international brain database. Int J Neurosci 117: 549–568 10.1080/00207450600773665
    1. Clark CR, Paul RH, Williams LM, Arns M, Fallahpour K, et al. (2006) Standardized assessment of cognitive functioning during development and aging using an automated touchscreen battery. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 21: 449–467 10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.005
    1. Arns M, Gunkelman J, Breteler M, Spronk D (2008) EEG phenotypes predict treatment outcome to stimulants in children with ADHD. J Integr Neurosci 7: 421–438.
    1. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55: 468–484.
    1. Kirk RE (1996) Practical Significance: A Concept Whose Time Has Come. Educational and Psychological Measurement 56: 746–759 10.1177/0013164496056005002
    1. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112: 155–159.
    1. Salthouse TA (2010) Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16: 754–760 10.1017/S1355617710000706
    1. Brickman AM, Meier IB, Korgaonkar MS, Provenzano FA, Grieve SM, et al. (2012) Testing the white matter retrogenesis hypothesis of cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1699–1715 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.001
    1. Daffner KR, Chong H, Sun X, Tarbi EC, Riis JL, et al. (2011) Mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 1298–1314 10.1162/jocn.2010.21540
    1. Salthouse TA (1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 103: 403–428.
    1. Davatzikos C, Resnick SM (2002) Degenerative age changes in white matter connectivity visualized in vivo using magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 12: 767–771.
    1. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA (2008) Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17: 177–182 10.1111/j.14678721.2008.00570.x
    1. Schneider-Garces NJ, Gordon BA, Brumback-Peltz CR, Shin E, Lee Y, et al. (2010) Span, CRUNCH, and beyond: working memory capacity and the aging brain. J Cogn Neurosci 22: 655–669 10.1162/jocn.2009.21230
    1. Vermeij A, van Beek AH, Olde Rikkert MG, Claassen JA, Kessels RP (2012) Effects of aging on cerebral oxygenation during working-memory performance: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One 7: e46210 10.1371/journal.pone.0046210
    1. Buckner RL (2004) Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44: 195–208 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006
    1. Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR (2002) Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults. Neuroimage 17: 1394–1402 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280
    1. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P (2009) The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 173–196 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656
    1. Davis SW, Dennis NA, Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R (2008) Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb Cortex 18: 1201–1209 10.1093/cercor/bhm155
    1. Friedman D, Kazmerski V, Fabiani M (1997) An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 104: 498–513.
    1. West R, Schwarb H, Johnson BN (2010) The influence of age and individual differences in executive function on stimulus processing in the oddball task. Cortex 46: 550–563 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.001
    1. Li L, Gratton C, Fabiani M, Knight RT (2013) Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention: an ERP study. Neurobiol Aging 34: 477–488 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025
    1. O’Connell RG, Balsters JH, Kilcullen SM, Campbell W, Bokde AW, et al. (2012) A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiol Aging 33: 2448–2461 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021

Source: PubMed

Подписаться