Decoding Spontaneous Emotional States in the Human Brain

Philip A Kragel, Annchen R Knodt, Ahmad R Hariri, Kevin S LaBar, Philip A Kragel, Annchen R Knodt, Ahmad R Hariri, Kevin S LaBar

Abstract

Pattern classification of human brain activity provides unique insight into the neural underpinnings of diverse mental states. These multivariate tools have recently been used within the field of affective neuroscience to classify distributed patterns of brain activation evoked during emotion induction procedures. Here we assess whether neural models developed to discriminate among distinct emotion categories exhibit predictive validity in the absence of exteroceptive emotional stimulation. In two experiments, we show that spontaneous fluctuations in human resting-state brain activity can be decoded into categories of experience delineating unique emotional states that exhibit spatiotemporal coherence, covary with individual differences in mood and personality traits, and predict on-line, self-reported feelings. These findings validate objective, brain-based models of emotion and show how emotional states dynamically emerge from the activity of separable neural systems.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Distributed patterns of brain activity…
Fig 1. Distributed patterns of brain activity predict the experience of discrete emotions.
(A) Parametric maps indicate brain regions in which increased fMRI signal informs the classification of emotional states. See [13] for details of the development and validation of these neural decoding models. (B) Sensitivity of the seven models. Error bars depict 95% confidence intervals. The data underlying this figure can be found in S1 Data.
Fig 2. Emotional states emerge spontaneously during…
Fig 2. Emotional states emerge spontaneously during resting-state scans.
(A) Procedure for classification of resting-state data. Scores are computed by taking the scalar product of preprocessed data and regression weights from decoding models. (B) Frequency distributions for the classification of all seven emotional states (n = 499). The mean, 25th, and 75th percentiles are indicated by black lines. The solid gray line indicates the number of trials that would occur from random guessing. The data underlying this figure can be found in S1 Data. The raw fMRI resting state data can be obtained from https://www.haririlab.com/projects.
Fig 3. Emotional states exhibit coherence during…
Fig 3. Emotional states exhibit coherence during resting-state scans.
Gray circles reflect the sample mean classification scores for all seven emotions (n = 499). Thick colored lines display group-average predicted time course using smoothing splines (with bordering 95% confidence interval). Text overlay (rcv) indicates the average cross-validated correlation between splines fitted for each subject and tested on the average fit of other subjects. Dashed lines indicate linear fits over time. Solid black dots indicate time points at which a model has the highest proportion of classifications. Data are concatenated across two sessions of 256 s (solid vertical line). Note the early peak for fear scores and general increases in neutral scores over time. The data underlying this figure can be found in S1 Data. The raw fMRI resting state data can be obtained from https://www.haririlab.com/projects.
Fig 4. Individual differences in mood and…
Fig 4. Individual differences in mood and personality modulate the occurrence of spontaneous emotional brain states.
(A) Differences in depressive and anxious mood are associated with increases in the frequency of sad and fear classifications during rest. (B) Emotional traits of Anxiety, Angry Hostility, and Depression track differences in the frequency of fear, anger, and sad classifications (n = 499, error bars reflect standard error, * indicates effects significant at Punc < .05). The data underlying this figure can be found in S1 Data. The raw fMRI resting state data can be obtained from https://www.haririlab.com/projects.
Fig 5. Spontaneous emotional brain states exhibit…
Fig 5. Spontaneous emotional brain states exhibit correspondence with self-report.
(A) Participants (n = 21) participated in an experience sampling task in which they reported their current emotional state at random intervals exceeding 30 s during fMRI scanning. The five samples of data (lasting 10 s, TR = 2 s) preceding each rating were used to compute predictions of emotional state using multivariate decoding models. (B) Scores for classification models congruent with self-report are greater than incongruent models (z = 2.311, Punc = 0.0208; Wilcoxon signed rank test). Classification scores are calculated based on the inner product of neural activity and classifier weights and indicate the relative evidence for the different emotion models. (C) The frequency of classifications from multivariate models significantly correlates with those made by participant self-report (r = .3876 ± 0.102 [s.e.m.], t20 = 2.537, Punc = .0196; one sample t test). Gray line indicates best-fitting least-squares line for group mean. In all panels, error bars reflect standard error of the mean. The data underlying this figure can be found in S1 Data.

References

    1. Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(9):424–30. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.005 .
    1. O'Toole AJ, Jiang F, Abdi H, Penard N, Dunlop JP, Parent MA. Theoretical, statistical, and practical perspectives on pattern-based classification approaches to the analysis of functional neuroimaging data. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(11):1735–52. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1735 .
    1. Haxby JV, Gobbini MI, Furey ML, Ishai A, Schouten JL, Pietrini P. Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science. 2001;293(5539):2425–30. 10.1126/science.1063736 .
    1. Kamitani Y, Tong F. Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(5):679–85. 10.1038/nn1444
    1. Tong F, Pratte MS. Decoding patterns of human brain activity. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:483–509. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100412 .
    1. Harrison SA, Tong F. Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature. 2009;458(7238):632–5. 10.1038/nature07832
    1. Serences JT, Ester EF, Vogel EK, Awh E. Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychol Sci. 2009;20(2):207–14. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02276.x
    1. Lewis-Peacock JA, Postle BR. Temporary activation of long-term memory supports working memory. J Neurosci. 2008;28(35):8765–71. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1953-08.2008
    1. Stokes M, Thompson R, Cusack R, Duncan J. Top-down activation of shape-specific population codes in visual cortex during mental imagery. J Neurosci. 2009;29(5):1565–72. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4657-08.2009 .
    1. Reddy L, Tsuchiya N, Serre T. Reading the mind's eye: decoding category information during mental imagery. Neuroimage. 2010;50(2):818–25. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.084
    1. Horikawa T, Tamaki M, Miyawaki Y, Kamitani Y. Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep. Science. 2013;340(6132):639–42. 10.1126/science.1234330 .
    1. Poldrack RA, Halchenko YO, Hanson SJ. Decoding the large-scale structure of brain function by classifying mental States across individuals. Psychol Sci. 2009;20(11):1364–72. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02460.x
    1. Kragel PA, LaBar KS. Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(11):1437–48. 10.1093/scan/nsv032
    1. Saarimaki H, Gotsopoulos A, Jaaskelainen IP, Lampinen J, Vuilleumier P, Hari R, et al. Discrete Neural Signatures of Basic Emotions. Cereb Cortex. 2016. June;26(6):2563–73. bhv086 [pii] 10.1093/cercor/bhv086 .
    1. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):700–11. Epub 2007/08/21. 10.1038/nrn2201 .
    1. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, et al. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature. 2007;447(7140):83–U4. 10.1038/Nature05758. ISI:000246149300048.
    1. Kenet T, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M, Grinvald A, Arieli A. Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature. 2003;425(6961):954–6. Epub 2003/10/31. 10.1038/nature02078 .
    1. Russell JA. A Circumplex Model of Affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161–78. 10.1037/H0077714. WOS:A1980KX30300015.
    1. DeNeve KM, Cooper H. The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(2):197–229. .
    1. Costa PT Jr., McCrae RR. Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: happy and unhappy people. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;38(4):668–78. .
    1. Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol. 2009;64(4):241–56. 10.1037/a0015309
    1. Chapman HA, Bernier D, Rusak B. MRI-related anxiety levels change within and between repeated scanning sessions. Psychiat Res-Neuroim. 2010;182(2):160–4. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.005. ISI:000278701500012.
    1. van Minde D, Klaming L, Weda H. Pinpointing Moments of High Anxiety During an MRI Examination. Int J Behav Med. 2014;21(3):487–95. 10.1007/s12529-013-9339-5. ISI:000335770800010.
    1. Muehlhan M, Lueken U, Wittchen HU, Kirschbaum C. The scanner as a stressor: Evidence from subjective and neuroendocrine stress parameters in the time course of a functional magnetic resonance imaging session. Int J Psychophysiol. 2011;79(2):118–26. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.09.009. ISI:000288294700005.
    1. Reinsch CH. Smoothing by spline functions. Numerische mathematik. 1967;10(3):177–83.
    1. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011
    1. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. 10.1177/014662167700100306
    1. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. 1970.
    1. Costa PT, McCrae RR. The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R). The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment. 2008;2:179–98.
    1. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Domains and Facets—Hierarchical Personality-Assessment Using the Revised NEO Personality-Inventory. J Pers Assess. 1995;64(1):21–50. 10.1207/s15327752jpa6401_2. ISI:A1995QE82600002.
    1. Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Vernon PA. Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. J Pers. 1996;64(3):577–91. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x. ISI:A1996VD76400002.
    1. McCrae RR, Costa PT. Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am Psychol. 1997;52(5):509–16. 10.1037/0003-066x.52.5.509. ISI:A1997WW85400001.
    1. Polyn SM, Natu VS, Cohen JD, Norman KA. Category-specific cortical activity precedes retrieval during memory search. Science. 2005;310(5756):1963–6. 10.1126/science.1117645 .
    1. Oatley K, Johnson-Laird PN. Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and emotion. 1987;1(1):29–50.
    1. Ekman P, Cordaro D. What is Meant by Calling Emotions Basic. Emot Rev. 2011;3(4):364–70. 10.1177/1754073911410740. WOS:000306274900002.
    1. Pessoa L. On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(2):148–58. WOS:000252503300016. 10.1038/nrn2317
    1. Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, Barrett LF. The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav Brain Sci. 2012;35(3):121–43. Epub 2012/05/24. 10.1017/S0140525X11000446
    1. Scherer KR. Emotions are emergent processes: they require a dynamic computational architecture. Philos T R Soc B. 2009;364(1535):3459–74. WOS:000271333900004.
    1. Eryilmaz H, De Ville DV, Schwartz S, Vuilleumier P. Lasting Impact of Regret and Gratification on Resting Brain Activity and Its Relation to Depressive Traits. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(23):7825–35. WOS:000337630700011. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0065-14.2014
    1. Eryilmaz H, Van De Ville D, Schwartz S, Vuilleumier P. Impact of transient emotions on functional connectivity during subsequent resting state: A wavelet correlation approach. Neuroimage. 2011;54(3):2481–91. WOS:000286302000070. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.021
    1. Kragel PA, LaBar KS. Decoding the Nature of Emotion in the Brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2016. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.011 .
    1. Touroutoglou A, Lindquist KA, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. Intrinsic connectivity in the human brain does not reveal networks for ‘basic’ emotions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2015;10(9):1257–65. 10.1093/scan/nsv013
    1. Vytal K, Hamann S. Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(12):2864–85. Epub 2009/11/26. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21366 .
    1. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349–56. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
    1. Kriegeskorte N, Cusack R, Bandettini P. How does an fMRI voxel sample the neuronal activity pattern: Compact-kernel or complex spatiotemporal filter? Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):1965–76. WOS:000273626400005. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.059
    1. Woo CW, Koban L, Kross E, Lindquist MA, Banich MT, Ruzic L, et al. Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection. Nature communications. 2014;5:5380 10.1038/ncomms6380
    1. Baumeister RF. A Self-Presentational View of Social Phenomena. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;91(1):3–26. WOS:A1982NF12700001.
    1. Kassam KS, Mendes WB. The effects of measuring emotion: physiological reactions to emotional situations depend on whether someone is asking. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(7):e64959 10.1371/journal.pone.0064959
    1. Salzman CD, Fusi S. Emotion, cognition, and mental state representation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:173–202. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135256
    1. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20:22–33;quiz 4–57. .
    1. M B First, Spitzer, Robert L, Gibbon Miriam, and Williams, Janet B.W. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; 1996.
    1. Chikazoe J, Lee DH, Kriegeskorte N, Anderson AK. Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(8):1114–22. Epub 2014/06/24. 10.1038/nn.3749
    1. Tusche A, Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Singer T. Classifying the wandering mind: revealing the affective content of thoughts during task-free rest periods. Neuroimage. 2014;97:107–16. Epub 2014/04/08. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.076 .
    1. Scherer KR. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information. 2005;44(4):695–729. 10.1177/0539018405058216. ISI:000233359100005.
    1. Wold S, Sjostrom M, Eriksson L. PLS-regression: a basic tool of chemometrics. Chemometr Intell Lab. 2001;58(2):109–30. 10.1016/S0169-7439(01)00155-1. ISI:000172360800006.
    1. Bullmore E, Long C, Suckling J, Fadili J, Calvert G, Zelaya F, et al. Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: resampling methods in time and wavelet domains. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;12(2):61–78. .
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate—a Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1995;57(1):289–300. ISI:A1995QE45300017.
    1. Yekutieli D, Benjamini Y. Resampling-based false discovery rate controlling multiple test procedures for correlated test statistics. J Stat Plan Infer. 1999;82(1–2):171–96. WOS:000083580500015.
    1. Landis JR, Koch GG. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–74. 10.2307/2529310

Source: PubMed

Подписаться