Influence of body mobility on attention networks in school-aged prematurely born children: A controlled trial

Joëlle Rosenbaum, Hadrien Ceyte, Isabelle Hamon, Hélène Deforge, Alexandre M J Hascoët, Sébastien Caudron, Jean-Michel Hascoët, Joëlle Rosenbaum, Hadrien Ceyte, Isabelle Hamon, Hélène Deforge, Alexandre M J Hascoët, Sébastien Caudron, Jean-Michel Hascoët

Abstract

School-aged prematurely born children (PC) have a higher risk of academic difficulties, which may be partly explained by attention difficulties. It has been suggested that children's attentional performance might be influenced by their body posture and spontaneous body motion. The aim of this study (ClinicalTrials.gov - NCT03125447) was to test the influence of three body mobility conditions on the three functions of attention (alertness, orienting, and executive control) among school-aged PC vs. term-born children (TC). Notably, 21 PC and 21 TC performed the Attention Network Test for Children in three body mobility conditions, namely, sitting and standing imposed fixed postures and a free-to-move condition. The children's median reaction times were compared between trials (1) with and without alerting cues, (2) with valid and invalid orienting cues, and (3) with and without distracting information, to calculate the performance of alertness, orienting, and executive control, respectively. Results showed that with distracting information, PC exhibited significantly slower responses in the standing-still posture than in the sitting-still posture (1,077 ± 240 vs. 1,175 ± 273 ms, p < 0.05), but not TC. No difference was observed with the free-to-move condition. PC and TC did not significantly differ in alertness or orienting, regardless of body mobility condition. These data suggest that PC must use executive resources to stand still and maintain position, which impairs their performance during executive tasks. We speculate that these results may be related to less developed postural control and motor inhibition in PC.

Keywords: alertness; body mobility; executive control; orienting; posture; prematurity.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Copyright © 2022 Rosenbaum, Ceyte, Hamon, Deforge, Hascoët, Caudron and Hascoët.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Mean of median RT for PC and TC in trials with congruent and incongruent targets in the three body mobility conditions. Error bars represent the mean absolute difference. *p < 0.05. TC, term-born children; PC, prematurely born children.

References

    1. Larroque B, Ancel PY, Marret S, Marchand L, André M, Arnaud C, et al. Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet. (2008) 371:813–20. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60380-3
    1. Larroque B, Ancel PY, Marchand-Martin L, Cambonie G, Fresson J, Pierrat V, et al. Special care and school difficulties in 8-year-old very preterm children: the epipage cohort study. PLoS One. (2011) 6:e21361. 10.1371/journal.pone.0021361
    1. Delobel-Ayoub M, Arnaud C, White-Koning M, Casper C, Pierrat V, Garel M, et al. Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of age after very preterm birth: the epipage study. Pediatrics. (2009) 123:1485–92. 10.1542/peds.2008-1216s
    1. Erdei C, Austin NC, Cherkerzian S, Morris AR, Woodward LJ. Predicting School-Aged Cognitive Impairment in Children Born Very Preterm. Pediatrics. (2020) 145:e20191982. 10.1542/peds.2019-1982
    1. Twilhaar ES, de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Academic trajectories of very preterm born children at school age. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2019) 104:F419–23. 10.1136/archdischild-2018-315028
    1. Twilhaar ES, de Kieviet JF, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Academic performance of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2018) 103:F322–30. 10.1136/archdischild-2017-312916
    1. McBryde M, Fitzallen GC, Liley HG, Taylor HG, Bora S. Academic outcomes of school-aged children born preterm: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Netw Open. (2020) 3:e202027. 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.2027
    1. Taylor R, Pascoe L, Scratch S, Doyle LW, Anderson P, Roberts G. A simple screen performed at school entry can predict academic under-achievement at age seven in children born very preterm: academic impairment and prematurity. J Paediat Child Health. (2016) 52:759–64. 10.1111/jpc.13186
    1. Akshoomoff N, Joseph RM, Taylor HG, Allred EN, Heeren T, O’Shea TM, et al. Academic achievement deficits and their neuropsychological correlates in children born extremely preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2017) 38:627–37. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000479
    1. Jaekel J, Wolke D, Bartmann P. Poor attention rather than hyperactivity/impulsivity predicts academic achievement in very preterm and full-term adolescents. Psychol Med. (2013) 43:183–96. 10.1017/S0033291712001031
    1. Leijon I, Ingemansson F, Nelson N, Wadsby M, Samuelsson S. Reading deficits in very low birthweight children are associated with vocabulary and attention issues at the age of seven. Acta Paediat. (2016) 105:60–8. 10.1111/apa.13094
    1. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. Modeling a cascade of effects: the role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement: executive function in preterm adolescents. Develop Sci. (2011) 14:1161–75. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01068.x
    1. Loe IM, Lee ES, Luna B, Feldman HM. Executive function skills are associated with reading and parent-rated child function in children born prematurely. Early Hum Develop. (2012) 88:111–8. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.07.018
    1. de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Lafeber HN, Oosterlaan J. Attention Problems of Very Preterm Children Compared with Age-Matched Term Controls at School-Age. J Pediat. (2012) 161:824–9. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.010
    1. Pizzo R, Urben S, Van Der Linden M, Borradori-Tolsa C, Freschi M, Forcada-Guex M, et al. Attentional networks efficiency in preterm children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2010) 16:130–7. 10.1017/S1355617709991032
    1. Giordano V, Fuiko R, Leiss U, Brandstetter S, Hayde M, Bartha-Doering E, et al. Differences in attentional functioning between preterm and full-term children underline the importance of new neuropsychological detection techniques. Acta Paediat. (2017) 106:601–11. 10.1111/apa.13723
    1. Geldof CJ, de Kieviet JF, Dik M, Kok JH, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis AG, Oosterlaan J. Visual search and attention in five-year-old very preterm/very low birth weight children. Early Hum Develop. (2013) 89:983–8. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.08.021
    1. Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N. Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: a systematic review. Develop Neuropsychol. (2009) 34:393–421. 10.1080/87565640902964524
    1. Réveillon M, Hüppi PS, Barisnikov K. Inhibition difficulties in preterm children: developmental delay or persistent deficit? Child Neuropsychol. (2018) 24:734–62. 10.1080/09297049.2017.1294665
    1. Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Attention problems in very preterm children from childhood to adulthood: the bavarian longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiat. (2016) 57:132–40. 10.1111/jcpp.12456
    1. Murray AL, Thompson DK, Pascoe L, Leemans A, Inder TE, Doyle LW, et al. White matter abnormalities and impaired attention abilities in children born very preterm. NeuroImage. (2016) 124:75–84. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.044
    1. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Ann Rev Neurosci. (1990) 13:25–42. 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
    1. Raz A, Buhle J. Typologies of attentional networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2006) 7:367–79. 10.1038/nrn1903
    1. Rueda MR, Posner MI. Development of Attention Networks. In: The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology (vol 1): Body and Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; (2013). p. 683–705.
    1. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1029–40. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012
    1. Mezzacappa E. Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Develop. (2004) 75:1373–86. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x
    1. Ishigami Y, Klein RM. Repeated measurement of the components of attention with young children using the attention network test: stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. J Cogn Develop. (2015) 16:144–59. 10.1080/15248372.2013.803971
    1. Lewis FC, Reeve RA, Johnson KA. A longitudinal analysis of the attention networks in 6-to 11-year-old children. Child Neuropsychol. (2018) 24:145–65. 10.1080/09297049.2016.1235145
    1. Mullane JC, Lawrence MA, Corkum PV, Klein RM, McLaughlin EN. The development of and interaction among alerting, orienting, and executive attention in children. Child Neuropsychol. (2016) 22:155–76. 10.1080/09297049.2014.981252
    1. Pozuelos JP, Paz-Alonso PM, Castillo A, Fuentes LJ, Rueda MR. Development of attention networks and their interactions in childhood. Develop Psychol. (2014) 50:2405–15. 10.1037/a0037469
    1. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. (2002) 14:340–7. 10.1162/089892902317361886
    1. Rosenbaum J, Hascoët JM, Hamon I, Petel A, Caudron S, Ceyte H. Body mobility and attention networks in 6-to 7-year-old children. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:743504. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.743504
    1. Eaton WO, McKeen NA, Campbell DW. The waxing and waning of movement: implications for psychological development. Develop Rev. (2001) 21:205–23. 10.1006/drev.2000.0519
    1. Krasner AJ, Turner JB, Feldman JF, Silberman AE, Fisher PW, Workman CC, et al. ADHD symptoms in a non-referred low birthweight/preterm cohort: longitudinal profiles, outcomes, and associated features. J Attent Disord. (2018) 22:827–38. 10.1177/1087054715617532
    1. de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Oosterlaan J, Visscher C, Hartman E. Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. (2018) 21:501–7. 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595
    1. Janssen M, Toussaint HM, van Mechelen W, Verhagen EA. Effects of acute bouts of physical activity on children’s attention: a systematic review of the literature. Springerplus. (2014) 3:410. 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.07.001
    1. Abou Khalil G, Doré-Mazars K, Senot P, Wang DP, Legrand A. Is it better to sit down, stand up or walk when performing memory and arithmetic activities? Exp Brain Res. (2020) 238:2487–96. 10.1007/s00221-020-05858-z
    1. Barra J, Auclair L, Charvillat A, Vidal M, Pérennou D. Postural control system influences intrinsic alerting state. Neuropsychology. (2015) 29:226–34. 10.1037/neu0000174
    1. Caldwell JA, Prazinko B, Caldwell JL. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. (2003) 114:23–31. 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00283-3
    1. Smith KC, Davoli CC, Knapp WH, Abrams RA. Standing enhances cognitive control and alters visual search. Attent Percept Psychophys. (2019) 81:2320–9. 10.3758/s13414-019-01723-6
    1. Rosenbaum D, Mama Y, Algom D. Stand by your Stroop: standing up enhances selective attention and cognitive control. Psychol Sci. (2017) 28:1864–7. 10.1177/0956797617721270
    1. Caron EE, Reynolds MG, Ralph BCW, Carriere JSA, Besner D, Smilek D. Does posture influence the stroop effect? Psychol Sci. (2020) 31:1452–60. 10.1177/0956797620953842
    1. Straub ER, Dames H, Kiesel A, Dignath D. Does body posture reduce the Stroop effect? Evidence from two conceptual replications and a meta-analysis. Acta Psychol. (2022) 224:103497. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103497
    1. Rapport MD, Bolden J, Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Raiker JS, Alderson RM. Hyperactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a ubiquitous core symptom or manifestation of working memory deficits? J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2009) 37:521–34. 10.1007/s10802-008-9287-8
    1. Patros CHG, Alderson RM, Hudec KL, Tarle SJ, Lea SE. Hyperactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the influence of underlying visuospatial working memory and self-control processes. J Exp Child Psychol. (2017) 154:1–12. 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.09.008
    1. Thibault RT, Raz A. Imaging posture veils neural signals. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:520. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00520
    1. Schaefer S, Lövdén M, Wieckhorst B, Lindenberger U. Cognitive performance is improved while walking: differences in cognitive–sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. Eur J Develop Psychol. (2010) 7:371–89. 10.1080/17405620802535666
    1. Ceyte H, Rosenbaum J, Hamon I, Wirth M, Caudron S, Hascoët JM. Mobility may impact attention abilities in healthy term or prematurely born children at 7-years of age: protocol for an intervention controlled trial. BMC Pediat. (2018) 18:264. 10.1186/s12887-018-1229-1
    1. Schneider BA, Avivi-Reich M, Mozuraitis M. A cautionary note on the use of the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in classification designs with and without within-subject factors. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:474. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00474
    1. Olivier I, Cuisinier R, Vaugoyeau M, Nougier V, Assaiante C. Age-related differences in cognitive and postural dual-task performance. Gait Post. (2010) 32:494–9. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.07.008
    1. Zhu JL, Olsen J, Olesen AW. Risk for developmental coordination disorder correlates with gestational age at birth. Paediat Perinat Epidemiol. (2012) 26:572–7. 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2012.01316.x
    1. van Hoorn JF, Schoemaker MM, Stuive I, Dijkstra PU, Rodrigues Trigo Pereira F, Van der Sluis CK, et al. Risk factors in early life for developmental coordination disorder: a scoping review. Develop Med Child Neurol. (2021) 63:511–9. 10.1111/dmcn.14781
    1. de Rose P, Albamonte E, Laganà V, Sivo S, Pisoni S, Gallini F, et al. Perceptual-motor abilities in pre-school preterm children. Early Hum Develop. (2013) 89:809–14. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.07.001
    1. de Kieviet JF, Piek JP, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Oosterlaan J. Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-weight children from birth to adolescence: a meta-analysis. JAMA. (2009) 302:2235–42. 10.1001/jama.2009.1708
    1. de Kieviet JF, Stoof CJJ, Geldof CJA, Smits N, Piek JP, Lafeber HN, et al. The crucial role of the predictability of motor response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Develop Med Child Neurol. (2013) 55:624–30. 10.1111/dmcn.12125
    1. Bolk J, Farooqi A, Hafström M, Åden U, Serenius F. Developmental coordination disorder and its association with developmental comorbidities at 6.5 years in apparently healthy children born extremely preterm. JAMA Pediat. (2018) 172:765–74. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1394
    1. Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D. EPICure Study Group. Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics. (2007) 120:793–804. 10.1542/peds.2007-0440
    1. Rodríguez Fernández C, Mata Zubillaga D, Rodríguez Fernández LM, Regueras Santos L, Reguera García MM, de Paz Fernández JA, et al. Evaluation of coordination and balance in preterm children. An Pediatr. (2016) 85:86–94. 10.1016/j.anpede.2015.10.023
    1. Lorefice LE, Galea MP, Clark RA, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Spittle AJ. Postural control at 4 years in very preterm children compared with term-born peers. Develop Med Child Neurol. (2015) 57:175–80. 10.1111/dmcn.12550
    1. Bucci MP, Tringali M, Trousson C, Husson I, Baud O, Biran V. Spatial and temporal postural analysis in children born prematurely. Gait Post. (2017) 57:230–5. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.06.023
    1. Bucci MP, Wiener-Vacher S, Trousson C, Baud O, Biran V. Subjective visual vertical and postural capability in children born prematurely. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0121616. 10.1371/journal.pone.0121616
    1. Kluenter H, Roedder D, Kribs A, Fricke O, Roth B, Guntinas-Lichius O. Postural control at 7 years of age after preterm birth with very low birth weight. Otol Neurotol. (2008) 29:1171–5. 10.1097/MAO.0b013e31818a0f5c
    1. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Smidts DP, Oosterlaan J, Duivenvoorden HJ, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2009) 37:981–93. 10.1007/s10802-009-9327-z
    1. Huertas F, Zahonero J, Sanabria D, Lupiáñez J. Functioning of the attentional networks at rest vs. during acute bouts of aerobic exercise. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2011) 33:649–65. 10.1123/jsep.33.5.649
    1. Assaiante C, Amblard B. An ontogenetic model for the sensorimotor organization of balance control in humans. Hum Move Sci. (1995) 14:13–43. 10.1016/0167-9457(94)00048-J
    1. Bonnet CT, Baudry S. Active vision task and postural control in healthy, young adults: synergy and probably not duality. Gait Post. (2016) 48:57–63. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.04.016
    1. Smith LK, Draper ES, Manktelow BN, Dorling JS, Field DJ. Socioeconomic inequalities in very preterm birth rates. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2007) 92:F11–4. 10.1136/adc.2005.090308

Source: PubMed

Подписаться