Food seeking in spite of harmful consequences is under prefrontal cortical noradrenergic control

Emanuele Claudio Latagliata, Enrico Patrono, Stefano Puglisi-Allegra, Rossella Ventura, Emanuele Claudio Latagliata, Enrico Patrono, Stefano Puglisi-Allegra, Rossella Ventura

Abstract

Background: Eating disorders are multifactorial psychiatric disorders. Chronic stressful experiences and caloric restriction are the most powerful triggers of eating disorders in human and animals. Although compulsive behavior is considered to characterize pathological excessive food intake, to our knowledge, no evidence has been reported of continued food seeking/intake despite its possible harmful consequences, an index of compulsive behavior. Brain monoamine transmission is considered to have a key role in vulnerability to eating disorders, and norepinephrine in medial prefrontal cortex has been shown to be critical for food-related motivated behavior.Here, using a new paradigm of conditioned suppression, we investigated whether the ability of a foot-shock-paired conditioned stimulus to suppress chocolate-seeking behavior was reversed by previous exposure to a food restriction experience, thus modeling food seeking in spite of harmful consequences in mice. Moreover, we assessed the effects of selective norepinephrine inactivation in medial prefrontal cortex on conditioned suppression test in stressed and caloric restricted mice.

Results: While Control (non food deprived) animals showed a profound conditioned suppression of chocolate seeking during presentation of conditioned stimulus, previously food restricted animals showed food seeking/intake despite its possible harmful consequences. Moreover, food seeking in spite of harmful consequences was prevented by selective norepinephrine inactivation, thus showing that prefrontal cortical norepinephrine is critical also for maladaptive food-related behavior.

Conclusions: These findings indicate that adaptive food seeking/intake can be transformed into maladaptive behaviors and point to "top-down" influence on eating disturbances and to new targets for therapy of aberrant eating behaviors.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Conditioned Suppression Test in Control and FD animals. Panel A. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) during training phase. Data are expressed as mean of four days of training. Panel B. Mean (sec ± SE) time (total time) spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) (when the aversive CS (light) was also present in the CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) during Conditioned Suppression Test (B1) by Control and FD animals. Striped pattern show partial time spent in CC and E-SC during Test by two groups; * p < 0.005 in comparison with empty-safe chamber. Chocolate intake shown by Control and FD groups on conditioned suppression test day (B2). Data are expressed as total mean grams ± SE. $ p < 0.005 in comparison with Control group.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Conditioned Suppression Test in Sham FD and NE depleted FD animals. Panel A. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) during training phase. Data are expressed as mean of four days of training. Panel B. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) (when the aversive CS (light) was also present in the CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) in Conditioned Suppression Test (B1) by Sham FD and NE depleted FD animals. Striped pattern show partial time spent in CC and E-SC during Test by two groups; * p < 0.005 in comparison with empty-safe chamber. Chocolate intake shown by Sham FD and NE depleted FD groups on conditioned suppression test day (B2). Data are expressed as total mean grams ± SE.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Conditioned Suppression Test in Sham Control and NE depleted Control animals. Panel A. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) during training phase. Data are expressed as mean of four days of training. Panel B. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in chamber containing chocolate (CC) (when the aversive CS (light) was also present in the CC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) in Conditioned Suppression Test (B1) by Sham Control and NE depleted Control animals; * p < 0.005 in comparison with empty-safe chamber. Chocolate intake shown by Sham Control and NE depleted Control groups on conditioned suppression test day (B2). Data are expressed as total mean grams ± SE.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Conditioned Avoidance Test in FD animals. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in the conditioned stimulus-paired chamber (CS-PC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) by FD animals; * p < 0.05 in comparison with CS-PC.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Conditioned Avoidance Test in Sham FD and NE depleted FD animals. Mean (sec ± SE) time spent in the conditioned stimulus-paired chamber (CS-PC) and in empty-safe chamber (E-SC) by Sham and NE depleted animals; * p < 0.05 in comparison with CS-PC.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Shock sensitivity in Sham FD and NE Depleted FD animals. Mean (μA ± SE) shock threshold observed in Sham FD and NE Depleted FD animals.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Schematic time-line of the experimental procedures (experiment 1 and experiment 2).

References

    1. Zandian M, Ioakimidis I, Bergh C, Södersten P. Cause and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Phys Behav. 2007;92:283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.052.
    1. Kaye W. Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Phys Behav. 2008;94:121–135. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.037.
    1. Casper RC, Sullivan EL, Tecott L. Relevance of animal models to human eating disorders and obesity. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:313–329. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1102-2.
    1. Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, La Fleur SE, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Bell ME, Bhatnagar S, Laugero KD, Manalo S. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of "comfort food". Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:11696–11701. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934666100.
    1. Hagan MM, Chandler PC, Wauford PK, Rybak RJ, Oswald KD. The role of palatable food and hunger as trigger factors in an animal model of stress induced binge eating. Int J Eat Disord. 2003;34:183–197. doi: 10.1002/eat.10168.
    1. Adam TC, Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward system. Phys Behav. 2007;91:449–458. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011.
    1. Wang G-J, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, Fowler JS. Similarity between obesity and drug addiction as assessed by neurofunctional imaging: a concept review. J Addict Dis. 2004;23:9–53. doi: 10.1300/J069v23n03_04.
    1. Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3306–3311.
    1. Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, Valdez J, Ragland D. Images of desire: food-craving activation during fMRI. NeuroImage. 2004;23:1486–1493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023.
    1. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:555–560. doi: 10.1038/nn1452.
    1. Fallon S, Shearman E, Sershen H, Lajtha A. Food reward-induced neurotransmitter changes in cognitive brain regions. Neurochem Res. 2007;32:1772–1782. doi: 10.1007/s11064-007-9343-8.
    1. Ventura R, Morrone C, Puglisi-Allegra S. Prefrontal/accumbal catecholamine system determines motivational salience attribution to both reward- and aversion-related stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:5181–5186. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610178104.
    1. Le Merrer J, Stephens DN. Food-induced behavioral sensitization, its cross-sensitization to cocaine and morphine, pharmacological blockade, and effect on food intake. J Neurosci. 2006;26:7163–7171. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5345-05.2006.
    1. Corwin RL, Buda-Levin A. Behavioral models of binge-type eating. Physiol Behav. 2007;82:123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.036.
    1. Lenoir M, Serre F, Cantin L, Ahmed SH. Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000698.
    1. Petrovich GD, Gallagher M. Control of food consumption by learned cues: A forebrain-hypothalamic network. Physiol Behav. 2007;91:397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.014.
    1. Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Holland PC, Gallagher M. Medial prefrontal cortex is necessary for an appetitive contextual conditioned stimulus to promote eating in sated rats. J Neurosci. 2007;27:6436–6441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5001-06.2007.
    1. Teegarden SL, Bale TL. Decreases in dietary preference produce increased emotionality and risk for dietary relapse. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:1021–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.032.
    1. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel B. Behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:20–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019.
    1. Coccurello R, D'Amato FR, Moles A. Chronic social stress, hedonism and vulnerability to obesity: Lessons from Rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:537–550. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.018.
    1. Deroche-Gamonet V, Belin D, Piazza PV. Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science. 2004;305:1014–1017. doi: 10.1126/science.1099020.
    1. Vanderschuren LJMJ, Everitt BJ. Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science. 2004;305:1017–1019. doi: 10.1126/science.1098975.
    1. Schroeder BE, Binzak JM, Kelley AE. A common profile of prefrontal cortical activation following exposure to nicotine- or chocolate-associated contextual cues. Neuroscience. 2001;105:535–545. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00221-4.
    1. Ghitza UE, Nair SG, Golden SA, Gray SM, Uejima JL, Bossert JM, Shaham Y. Peptide YY3-36 decreases reinstatement of high-fat food seeking during dieting in a rat relapse model. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11522–11532. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5405-06.2007.
    1. Parker G, Parker I, Brotchie H. Mood state effects of chocolate. J Affect Dis. 2006;92:149–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.007.
    1. Killgore WD, Young AD, Femia LA, Bogorodzki P, Rogowska J, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1381–1394. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00191-5.
    1. Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, Dalgleish T, Phillips ML, Ng VW, Andrew CM, Williams SC, Campbell IC, Treasure J. Medial prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1238–1246. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1238.
    1. Schiltz CA, Bremer QZ, Landry CF, Kelley AE. Food-associated cues alter forebrain functional connectivity as assessed with immediate early gene and proenkephalin expression. BMC Biology. 2007;5:16. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-5-16.
    1. Darracq L, Blanc G, Glowinski J, Tassin JP. Importance of the noradrenaline-dopamine coupling in the locomotor activating effects of D-amphetamine. J Neurosci. 1998;18:2729–2739.
    1. Feenstra MG, Botterblom MH, Mastenbroek S. Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the prefrontal cortex in the light and dark period: effects of novelty and handling and comparison to the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience. 2000;100:741–748. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00319-5.
    1. Mingote S, de Bruin JPC, Feesntra MGP. Noradrenaline and dopamine efflux in the prefrontal cortex in relation to appetitive classical conditioning. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2475–2480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4547-03.2004.
    1. Ventura R, Cabib S, Alcaro A, Orsini C, Puglisi-Allegra S. Norepinephrine in the prefrontal cortex is critical for amphetamine-induced reward and mesoaccumbens dopamine release. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1879–1885.
    1. Ventura R, Alcaro A, Puglisi-Allegra S. Prefrontal cortical norepinephrine release is critical for morphine-induced reward, reinstatement and dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1877–1886. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi066.
    1. Ventura R, Latagliata EC, Morrone C, La Mela I, Puglisi-Allegra S. Prefrontal norepinephrine determines attribution of "high" motivational salience. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e3044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003044.
    1. Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:407–419. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00033-1.
    1. Carr KD. Augmentation of drug reward by chronic food restriction: behavioral evidence and underlying mechanisms. Physiol Behav. 2002;76:353–364. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00759-X.
    1. Cabib S, Orsini C, Le Moal M, Piazza PV. Abolition and Reversal of Strain Differences in Behavioral Responses to Drugs of Abuse After a Brief Experience. Science. 2000;289:463–465. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5478.463.
    1. Rougé-Pont F, Marinelli M, Le Moal M, Simon H, Piazza PV. Stress-induced sensitization and glucocorticoids. II. Sensitization of the increase in extracellular dopamine induced by cocaine depends on stress-induced corticosterone secretion. J Neurosi. 1995;15:7189–7195.
    1. Deroche V, Marinelli M, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H, Piazza PV. Stress-induced sensitization and glucocorticoids. I. Sensitization of dopamine-dependent locomotor effects of amphetamine and morphine depends on stress-induced corticosterone secretion. J Neurosci. 1995;15:7181–71888.
    1. Armario A, Montero JL, Jolin T. Chronic food restriction and the circadian rhythms of pituitary-adrenal hormones, growth hormone and thyroid-stimulating hormone. Ann Nutr Metab. 1987;31:81–87. doi: 10.1159/000177254.
    1. García-Belenguer S, Oliver C, Mormède P. Facilitation and feedback in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis during food restriction in rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 1993;5:663–668. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1993.tb00537.x.
    1. Han ES, Evans TR, Nelson J. Adrenocortical responsiveness to adrenocorticotropic hormone is enhanced in chronically food-restricted rats. J Nutr. 1998;128:1415–1420.
    1. Johansson A, Fredriksson R, Winnergren S, Hulting AL, Schiöth HB, Lindblom J. The relative impact of chronic food restriction and acute food deprivation on plasma hormone levels and hypothalamic neuropeptide expression. Peptides. 2008;29:1588–1595. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.04.018.
    1. Stamp JA, Mashoodh R, van Kampen JM, Robertson HA. Food restriction enhances peak corticosterone levels, cocaine-induced locomotor activity, and DeltaFosB expression in the nucleus accumbens of the rat. Brain Res. 2008;1204:94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.019.
    1. Surwit RS, Feinglos MN, Rodin J, Sutherland A, Petro AE, Opara EC, Kuhn CM, Rebuffè-Scrive M. Differential effects of fat and sucrose on the development of obesità and diabetes in C57BL/6J and A/J mice. Metabolism. 1995;44:645–651. doi: 10.1016/0026-0495(95)90123-X.
    1. Shi WX, Pun CL, Zhang XX, Jones MD, Bunney BS. Dual effects of D-amphetamine on dopamine neurons mediated by dopamine and nondopamine receptors. J Neurosci. 2000;20:3504–3511.
    1. Peciña S, Schulkin J, Berridge KC. Nucleus accumbens corticotropin-releasing factor increases cue-triggered motivation for sucrose reward: paradoxical positive incentive effects in stress? BMC Biol. 2006;13(4):8. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-4-8.
    1. Koob GF. Corticotropin-Releasing Factor, Norepinephrine, and Stress. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1167–1180. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00164-X.
    1. Weinshenker D, Schroeder JPS. There and back again: a tale of norepinephrine and drug addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1433–1451. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301263.
    1. Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The Mouse Brain. Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic, San Diego) 1998.
    1. Pascucci T, Ventura R, Latagliata EC, Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. The medial prefrontal cortex determines the accumbens dopamine response to stress through the opposing influences of norepinephrine and dopamine. Cereb Cortex. 2007;12:2796–2804. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm008.
    1. Shalev U, Marinelli M, Baumann MH, Piazza PV, Shaham Y. The role of corticosterone in food deprivation-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:170–176. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1200-5.
    1. Beck KD, Luine VN. Food deprivation modulates chronic stress effects on object recognition in male rats: role of monoamines and amino acids. Brain Res. 1999;830:56–71. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01380-3.
    1. Ventura R, Puglisi-Allegra S. Environment makes amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens totally impulse-dependent. Synapse. 2005;58:211–214. doi: 10.1002/syn.20197.
    1. Spink AJ, Tegelenbosch RA, Buma MO, Noldus LP. The EthoVision video tracking system--a tool for behavioral phenotyping of transgenic mice. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:731–744. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00530-3.
    1. Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S, Ebel A, Kempf J, Castellano C. Passive avoidance behavior in mice: interaction between age and genotype. Exp Aging Res. 1986;12:107–109.

Source: PubMed

Подписаться