Immediate memory consequences of the effect of emotion on attention to pictures

Deborah Talmi, Adam K Anderson, Lily Riggs, Jeremy B Caplan, Morris Moscovitch, Deborah Talmi, Adam K Anderson, Lily Riggs, Jeremy B Caplan, Morris Moscovitch

Abstract

Emotionally arousing stimuli are at once both highly attention grabbing and memorable. We examined whether emotional enhancement of memory (EEM) reflects an indirect effect of emotion on memory, mediated by enhanced attention to emotional items during encoding. We tested a critical prediction of the mediation hypothesis-that regions conjointly activated by emotion and attention would correlate with subsequent EEM. Participants were scanned with fMRI while they watched emotional or neutral pictures under instructions to attend to them a lot or a little, and were then given an immediate recognition test. A region in the left fusiform gyrus was activated by emotion, voluntary attention, and subsequent EEM. A functional network, different for each attention condition, connected this region and the amygdala, which was associated with emotion and EEM, but not with voluntary attention. These findings support an indirect cortical mediation account of immediate EEM that may complement a direct modulation model.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Behavioral results. Corrected recognition (hits-FA, in percent, collapsed across confidence ratings) as a function of emotion and attention. Recognition was greater for emotional relative to neutral pictures, a difference larger in the low-attention condition. Error bars represent SE.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Regions associated with attention. Regions associated with attending neutral pictures (yellow), scrambled pictures (pink), and the spatial overlap between these contrasts (green). (A) Y = −64; the arrowheads point to the intraparietal sulcus. (B) Z = 49; the arrowheads point to the frontal eye field. (C) Z = −10.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Spatial overlap of attention and emotion. The overlap (pink) of emotion (red) and content-independent attention (green), and the overlap (purple) of emotion and content-dependent attention (yellow). (A) Y = −79; the arrowhead points to the intraparietal sulcus. (B) X = −47.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Regions associated with EEM (emotional Dm > neutral Dm). (A) Z = −23. (B) Z = −13. (C) Spatial dissociation in the left amygdala’s association with emotion and EEM, Y = −5. Dorsal amygdala correlated with emotion (red), while ventral amygdala correlated with EEM. These locations correspond to the central nucleus and the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, respectively.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Spatial overlap of emotion, content-dependent attention, and EEM. (A) Activations associated with attention (yellow), with emotion (red), with the overlap of attention and emotion (purple), and with the overlap of EEM with either emotion or attention (dark blue). The arrowhead points to the spatial overlap of emotion, content-dependent attention, and EEM (light blue). X = −44. (B) Additive effects of emotion and attention as a function of picture type in the cluster activated by emotion, attention, and EEM. Error bars represent SE.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Pearson R correlations of brain scores and seed values as a function of task condition in the first (top) and second (bottom) LV in the PLS analysis. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval based on a permutations test.

References

    1. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2002;12:169–177.
    1. Adolphs R., Tranel D., Buchanan T.W. Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:512–518.
    1. Algom D., Chaiut E., Lev S. A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2004;133:323–338.
    1. Amaral D.G., Price J.L. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis) J. Comp. Neurol. 1984;230:465–496.
    1. Amaral D.G., Behniea H., Kelly J.L. Topographic organization of projections from the amygdala to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 2003;118:1099–1120.
    1. Anderson A.K. Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2005;134:258–281.
    1. Anderson A.K., Phelps E.A. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature. 2001;411:305–309.
    1. Anderson A.K., Christoff K., Panitz D., De Rosa E., Gabrieli J.D. Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:5627–5633.
    1. Anderson A.K., Wais P.E., Gabrieli J.D. Emotion enhances remembrance of neutral events past. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2006;103:1599–1604.
    1. Bem S.L. Bem sex-role inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.; Redwood City, CA: 1981.
    1. Bianchin M., Mello e Souza T., Medina J.H., Izquierdo I. The amygdala is involved in the modulation of long-term memory, but not in working or short-term memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1999;71:127–131.
    1. Bingel U., Rose M., Glascher J., Buchel C. fMRI reveals how pain modulates visual object processing in the ventral visual stream. Neuron. 2007;55:157–167.
    1. Bradley M.M., Lang P.J. Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59.
    1. Bradley M.M., Sabatinelli D., Lang P.J., Fitzsimmons J.R., King W., Desai P. Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behav. Neurosci. 2003;117:369–380.
    1. Brown M.W., Aggleton J.P. Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:51–61.
    1. Buchel C., Friston K.J. Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: Cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling and fMRI. Cereb. Cortex. 1997;7:768–778.
    1. Cahill L., Alkire M.T. Epinephrine enhancement of human memory consolidation: Interaction with arousal at encoding. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2003;79:194–198.
    1. Cahill L., McGaugh J.L. Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:294–299.
    1. Cahill L., Babinsky R., Markowitsch H.J., McGaugh J.L. The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature. 1995;377:295–296.
    1. Cahill L., Haier R.J., White N.S., Fallon J., Kilpatrick L., Lawrence C., Potkin S.G., Alkire M.T. Sex-related difference in amygdala activity during emotionally influenced memory storage. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2001;75:1–9.
    1. Cahill L., Gorski L., Le K. Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learn. Mem. 2003;10:270–274.
    1. Cahill L., Gorski L., Belcher A., Huynh Q. The influence of sex versus sex-related traits on long-term memory for gist and detail from an emotional story. Conscious. Cogn. 2004a;13:391–400.
    1. Cahill L., Uncapher M., Kilpatrick L., Alkire M., Turner J. Sex-related hemispheric lateralization of amygdala function in emotionally influenced memory: An fMRI investigation. Learn. Mem. 2004b;11:261–266.
    1. Canli T., Zhao Z., Brewer J., Gabrieli J.D., Cahill L. Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:RC99. .
    1. Canli T., Desmond J.E., Zhao Z., Gabrieli J.D. Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2002;99:10789–10794.
    1. Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215.
    1. Cox R.W. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173.
    1. Cox R.W., Hyde J.S. Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data. NMR Biomed. 1997;10:171–178.
    1. Craik F.I., Govoni R., Naveh-Benjamin M., Anderson N.D. The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1996;125:159–180.
    1. Critchley H., Daly E., Phillips M., Brammer M., Bullmore E., Williams S., Van Amelsvoort T., Robertson D., David A., Murphy D. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2000;9:93–105.
    1. Desimone R., Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222.
    1. Dolan R.J. Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science. 2002;298:1191–1194.
    1. Dolcos F., Labar K.S., Cabeza R. Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron. 2004;42:855–863.
    1. Edgington E.S. Randomization tests. Marcel Dekker; New York: 1980.
    1. Efron B., Tibshirani R.J. An introduction to the bootstrap. Chapman & Hall, Inc.; New York: 1993.
    1. Epstein R., Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature. 1998;392:598–601.
    1. Friston K.J., Penny W.D., Glaser D.E. Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage. 2005;25:661–667.
    1. Gallagher M., Schoenbaum G. Functions of the amygdala and related forebrain areas in attention and cognition. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999;877:397–411.
    1. Glover G.H., Lai S. Self-navigated spiral fMRI: Interleaved versus single-shot. Magn. Reson. Med. 1998;39:361–368.
    1. Hadley C.B., Mackay D.G. Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2006;32:79–88.
    1. Hamann S. Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001;5:394–400.
    1. Hariri A.R., Bookheimer S.Y., Mazziotta J.C. Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport. 2000;11:43–48.
    1. Horwitz B., Friston K.J., Taylor J.G. Neural modeling and funcitonal brain imaging: An overview. Neural Netw. 2000;13:829–846.
    1. Izquierdo L.A., Barros D.M., Vianna M.R., Coitinho A., deDavid e Silva T., Choi H., Moletta B., Medina J.H., Izquierdo I. Molecular pharmacological dissection of short- and long-term memory. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2002;22:269–287.
    1. Kastner S., Ungerleider L.G. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;23:315–341.
    1. Keil A., Bradley M.M., Hauk O., Rockstroh B., Elbert T., Lang P.J. Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:641–649.
    1. Keil A., Moratti S., Sabatinelli D., Bradley M.M., Lang P.J. Additive effects of emotional content and spatial selective attention on electrocortical facilitation. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:1187–1197.
    1. Kensinger E.A., Corkin S. Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2004;101:3310–3315.
    1. Kern R.P., Libkuman T.M., Otani H., Holmes K. Emotional stimuli, divided attention, and memory. Emotion. 2005;5:408–417.
    1. LaBar K.S., Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:54–64.
    1. LaBar K.S., Phelps E.A. Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psychol. Sci. 1998;9:490–493.
    1. Lane R.D., Chua P.M., Dolan R.J. Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:989–997.
    1. Lang P.J., Bradley M.M., Fitzsimmons J.R., Cuthbert B.N., Scott J.D., Moulder B., Nangia V. Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: An fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:199–210.
    1. Lang P.J., Bradley M.M., Cuthbert B.N. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. University of Florida; Gainesville, FL: 2005.
    1. Lieberman M.D., Eisenberger N.I., Crockett M.J., Tom S.M., Pfeifer J.H., Way B.M. Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:421–428.
    1. Mackiewicz K.L., Sarinopoulos I., Cleven K.L., Nitschke J.B. The effect of anticipation and the specificity of sex differences for amygdala and hippocampus function in emotional memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2006;103:14200–14205.
    1. McGaugh J.L. Memory reconsolidation hypothesis revived but restrained: Theoretical comment on Biedenkapp and Rudy (2004) Behav. Neurosci. 2004;118:1140–1142.
    1. McIntosh A.R. Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. Memory. 1999;7:523–548.
    1. McIntosh A.R., Gonzalez-Lima F. Large-scale functional connectivity in associative learning: Interrelations of the rat auditory, visual, and limbic systems. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;80:3148–3162.
    1. McIntosh A.R., Lobaugh N.J. Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: Applications and advances. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S250–S263.
    1. McIntosh A.R., Bookstein F.L., Haxby J.V., Grady C.L. Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images using partial least squares. Neuroimage. 1996;3:143–157.
    1. Morris J.S., Friston K.J., Buchel C., Frith C.D., Young A.W., Calder A.J. A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain. 1998;121:47–57.
    1. Morris J.S., Ohman A., Dolan R.J. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 1999;96:1680–1685.
    1. Nichols T., Brett M., Andersson J., Wager T., Poline J.B. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage. 2005;25:653–660.
    1. Ogawa S., Lee T.M., Nayak A.S., Glynn P. Oxygenation-sensitive contrast in magnetic-resonance image of rodent brain at high magnetic-fields. Magn. Reson. Med. 1990;14:68–78.
    1. Olson I.R., Plotzker A., Ezzyat Y. The Enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain. 2007;130:1718–1731.
    1. Paller K.A., Wagner A.D. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002;6:93–102.
    1. Pessoa L., Kastner S., Ungerleider L.G. Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002;15:31–45.
    1. Phelps E.A., LaBar K.S., Anderson A.K., O'Connor K.J., Fulbright R.K., Spencer D.D. Specifying the contributions of the human amygdala to emotional memory: A case study. Neurocase. 1998;4:527–540.
    1. Pinsk M.A., Doniger G.M., Kastner S. Push-pull mechanism of selective attention in human extrastriate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;92:622–629.
    1. Pitkanen A. The amygdala: A functional analysis. (ed. J.P. Aggleton); Oxford University Press, New York: 2000. Connectivity of the rat amygdaloid complex; pp. 31–116.
    1. Posner M.I., Driver J. The neurobiology of selective attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1992;2:165–169.
    1. Pratto F., John O.P. Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991;61:380–391.
    1. Price J.L. Comparative aspects of amygdala connectivity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2003;985:50–58.
    1. Richardson M.P., Strange B.A., Dolan R.J. Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:278–285.
    1. Sabatinelli D., Lang P.J., Keil A., Bradley M.M. Emotional perception: Correlation of functional MRI and event-related potentials. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:1085–1091.
    1. Schimmack U. Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion. 2005;5:55–66.
    1. Schupp H.T., Stockburger J., Codispoti M., Junghofer M., Weike A.I., Hamm A.O. Selective visual attention to emotion. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:1082–1089.
    1. Sereno M.I., Pitzalis S., Martinez A. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal cortical area in humans. Science. 2001;294:1350–1354.
    1. Sharot T., Phelps E.A. How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;4:294–306.
    1. Sharot T., Delgado M.R., Phelps E.A. How emotion enhances the feeling of remembering. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:1376–1380.
    1. Strange B.A., Hurlemann R., Dolan R.J. An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and β-adrenergic-dependent. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2003;100:13626–13631.
    1. Talairach J., Tournaux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Thieme; Stuttgart, Germany: 1998.
    1. Talmi D., Schimmack U., Paterson T., Moscovitch M. The role of attention and relatedness in emotionally enhanced memory. Emotion. 2007;7:89–102.
    1. Thomas R.C., Hasher L. The influence of emotional valence on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychol. Aging. 2006;21:821–825.
    1. Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:585–594.
    1. Vuilleumier P., Driver J. Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: Windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2007;362:837–855.
    1. Vuilleumier P., Sagiv N., Hazeltine E., Poldrack R.A., Swick D., Rafal R.D., Gabrieli J.D. Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: A combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2001a;98:3495–3500.
    1. Vuilleumier P., Armony J.L., Driver J., Dolan R.J. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2001b;30:829–841.
    1. Vuilleumier P., Richardson M.P., Armony J.L., Driver J., Dolan R.J. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:1271–1278.
    1. Webster M.J., Bachevalier J., Ungerleider L.G. Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. Cereb. Cortex. 1994;4:470–483.

Source: PubMed

Подписаться