But do you think I'm cool? Developmental differences in striatal recruitment during direct and reflected social self-evaluations

Kathryn F Jankowski, William E Moore, Junaid S Merchant, Lauren E Kahn, Jennifer H Pfeifer, Kathryn F Jankowski, William E Moore, Junaid S Merchant, Lauren E Kahn, Jennifer H Pfeifer

Abstract

The current fMRI study investigates the neural foundations of evaluating oneself and others during early adolescence and young adulthood. Eighteen early adolescents (ages 11-14, M=12.6) and 19 young adults (ages 22-31, M=25.6) evaluated whether academic, physical, and social traits described themselves directly (direct self-evaluations), described their best friend directly (direct other-evaluations), described themselves from their best friend's perspective (reflected self-evaluations), or in general could change over time (control malleability-evaluations). Compared to control evaluations, both adolescents and adults recruited cortical midline structures during direct and reflected self-evaluations, as well as during direct other-evaluations, converging with previous research. However, unique to this study was a significant three-way interaction between age group, evaluative perspective, and domain within bilateral ventral striatum. Region of interest analyses demonstrated a significant evaluative perspective by domain interaction within the adolescent sample only. Adolescents recruited greatest bilateral ventral striatum during reflected social self-evaluations, which was positively correlated with age and pubertal development. These findings suggest that reflected social self-evaluations, made from the inferred perspective of a close peer, may be especially self-relevant, salient, or rewarding to adolescent self-processing--particularly during the progression through adolescence - and this feature persists into adulthood.

Keywords: Adolescence; Medial prefrontal cortex; Puberty; Self; Social cognition; Ventral striatum.

Copyright © 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Neuroimaging task. The neuroimaging task included two runs, with eight blocks per run. At the beginning of each block, participants were instructed to evaluate trait phrases according to a given perspective, followed by a series of nine trait phrases. Phrases were positively and negatively valenced and represented academic, physical, and social domains (all intermixed within blocks). In between evaluation blocks were blocks of rest. Note: The name of each participant's best friend was always included (e.g., “Lily”); s = seconds.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Main effect of evaluative perspective. Repeated measures ANOVA, where evaluative perspective and domain served as the within-subjects factors and age group served as the between-subjects factor. Illustrated here is a significant main effect of evaluative perspective within cortical midline structures (engaged by direct self-, direct other-, and reflected self-evaluations) and bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (primarily engaged by malleability-evaluations). Note: x and y = left–right and anterior–posterior dimensions.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Three-way interaction (age group × evaluative perspective × domain). Repeated measures ANOVA, where evaluative perspective and domain served as the within-subjects factors and age group served as the between-subjects factor. Illustrated here is a significant age group × evaluative perspective × domain interaction within bilateral ventral striatum. Note: x and y = left–right and anterior–posterior dimensions.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Mean parameter estimates in ventral striatum across evaluative conditions. Interaction between evaluative perspective and domain. Panel (A) illustrates mean parameter estimates representing activity in the left ventral striatum (VS) cluster during reflected social self-evaluations within the adult group. Panel (B) illustrates mean parameter estimates representing activity in the left VS cluster during reflected social self-evaluations within the adolescent group. Note: L = left; VS = ventral striatum.
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Positive relationship between pubertal development and striatal activity during adolescent reflected social self-evaluations. Correlation between average Pubertal Development Scale (PDS) scores and mean parameter estimates representing activity in left ventral striatum (VS) cluster during adolescent reflected social self-evaluations. Note: L = left; VS = ventral striatum.

References

    1. Blakemore S.-J. Development of the social brain during adolescence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2008;61(1):40–49.
    1. Blakemore S.-J. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9(4):267–277.
    1. Blakemore S.-J. Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage. 2012;61:397–406.
    1. Blakemore S.-J., Burnett S., Dahl R. The role of puberty in the development of the adolescent brain. Human Brain Development. 2010;31:926–933.
    1. Blakemore S.-J., den Ouden, Choudhury S., Frith C. Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2:30–139.
    1. Broughton J. Development of concepts of self, mind, reality, and knowledge. New Directions for Child Development. 1978;1:75–101.
    1. Buhrmester D. Need fulfillment, interpersonal competence, and the developmental contexts of early adolescent friendship. In: Bukowski W., Newcomb A., Hartup W., editors. The Company They Kee. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 1996. pp. 158–185.
    1. Burnett S., Bird G., Moll J., Frith C., Blakemore S.-J. Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(9):1736–1750.
    1. Camara E., Rodriguez-Fornells A., Münte T.F. Functional connectivity of reward processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2009;2(19):1–14.
    1. Choudhury S., Blakemore S.-J., Charman T. Social cognitive development during adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2006;1:165–174.
    1. Collins W.A., Russell G. Mother–child and father–child relationships in middle childhood and adolescence: a developmental analysis. Developmental Review. 1991;11:99–136.
    1. Csikszentmihalyi M., Larson R. Basic Books; New York, NY: 1984. Being Adolescent.
    1. Craik F.I.M., Moroz T.M., Moscovitch M., Stuff D.T., Winocur G., Tulving E., Kaupr S. In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science. 1999;10(1):26–34.
    1. Damon W., Hart D. The development of self-understanding from infancy through adolescence. Child Development. 1982;53(4):841–864.
    1. Damon W., Hart D. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 1988. Self-Understanding in Childhood and Adolescence.
    1. Davey C.G., Allen N.B., Harrison B.J., Dwyer D.B., Yucel M. Being liked activates primary reward and midline self-related brain regions. Human Brain Mapping. 2010;31:660–668.
    1. de Greck M., Rotte M., Paus R., Moritz D., Thiemann R., Proesch U., Bruer U., Moerth S., Tempelmann C., Bpgerts B., Northoff G. Is our self based on reward? Self-relatedness recruits neural activity in the reward system. Neuroimage. 2008;39:2066–2075.
    1. Delgado M.R., Locke H.M., Stenger V.A., Fiez J.A. Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: effects of valence and magnitude manipulations. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3(1):27–38.
    1. Dumontheil I., Apperly I.A., Blakemore S.-J. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science. 2009;13(2):331–338.
    1. Elkind D. Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development. 1967;38(4):1025–1034.
    1. Elkind D., Bowen D. Imaginary audience behavior in children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology. 1979;15:38–44.
    1. Elliot R., Friston K.J., Dolan R.J. Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:6159–6165.
    1. Enzi B., de Greck M., Prosch U., Tempelmann C., Northoff G. Is our self nothing but reward? Neuronal overlap and distinction between reward and personal relevance and its relation to human personality. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(12):e8429.
    1. Erikson . 2nd edition. Norton; New York, NY: 1963. Childhood and Society.
    1. Ersner-Hershfield H., Wimmer G.E., Knutson B. Saving for the future self: neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2009;4:85–92.
    1. Ernst M., Nelson E.E., McClure E.B., Monk C.S., Munson S., Eshel N., Zarahn E., Leibenluft E., Zametkin A., Towbin K., Blair J., Charney D., Pine D.S. Choice selection and reward anticipation: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1585–1597.
    1. Falk E.B., Way B.M., Jasinska A.J. An imaging genetics approach to understanding social influence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6(168):1–13.
    1. Fliessbach K., Weber B., Trautner P., Dohmen T., Sunde U., Elger C.E., Falk A. Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. Science. 2007;318:1305–1308.
    1. Galvan A. Adolescent development of the reward system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4(6):1–9.
    1. Gillihan S.J., Farah M.J. Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(1):76–97.
    1. Goldstein R.Z., Tomasi D., Akia-Klein N., Cottone L.A., Zhang L., Telang F., Volkow N.D. Subjective sensitivity to monetary gradients is associated with frontolimbic activation to reward in cocaine abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;87:233–240.
    1. Greenberg D.L., Rice H.J., Cooper J.J., Cabeza R., Rubin D.C., LaBar K.S. Co-activation of the amygdala, hippocampus, and inferior frontal gyrus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(5):659–674.
    1. Grinband J., Wager T.D., Lindquist M., Ferrera V.P., Hirsch J. Detection of time-varying signals in event-related fMRI designs. Neuroimage. 2008;43(3):509–520.
    1. Gunther Moor B., van Leijenhorst L., Rombouts S.A.R.B., Crone E.A., Van der Molen M.W. Do you like me? Neural correlates of social evaluation and developmental trajectories. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5(5–6):461–482.
    1. Gunther Moor B., Op de Macks Z.A., Güroglu B., Rombouts S.A.R.B., Van der Molen M.W., Crone E.A. Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7(1):44–52.
    1. Guyer A.E., McClure-Tone E.B., Shiffrin N.D., Pine D.S., Nelson E.E. Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Development. 2009;80(4):1000–1015.
    1. Guyer A.E., Choate V.R., Pine D.S., Nelson E.E. Neural circuitry underlying affective response to peer feedback in adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:81–92.
    1. Haber S.N., Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26.
    1. Harter S. Self and identity development. In: Feldman S.S., Elliot G.R., editors. At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1990. pp. 388–413.
    1. Harter S. The development of self-representations. In: Damon W., Eisenberg N., editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. 5th ed. Wiley; New York, NY: 1998. pp. 553–617.
    1. Harter S. Guilford; New York, NY: 1999. The Construction of the Self: A Developmental Perspective.
    1. Harter S., Waters P., Whitesell N.R. Relational self-worth: differences in perceived worth as a person across interpersonal contexts among adolescents. Child Development. 1998;69(3):756–766.
    1. Heatherton T.F., Wyland C.L., Macrae N.M., Demos K.E., Denny B.T., Kelly W.M. Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2006;1:18–25.
    1. Kaufman A.S., Kaufman N.L. 2nd edition. American Guidance Service Inc.; Circle Pines, MN: 2004. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test.
    1. Kelley W.M., Macrae C.N., Wyland C.L., Caglar S., Inati S., Heatherton T.F. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14(5):785–794.
    1. Kjaer T.W., Nowak M., Lou H.C. Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1080–1086.
    1. Kircher T.T.J., Brammer M., Bullmore E., Simmons A., Bartels M., David A.S. The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self-processing. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:683–692.
    1. Knutson B., Fong G.W., Adams C.M., Varner J.L., Hommer D. Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport. 2001;12:3683–3687.
    1. Knutson B., Taylor J., Kaufman M., Peterson R., Glover G. Distributed neural representation of expected value. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25(19):4806–4812.
    1. Knutson B., Westdorp A., Kaiser E., Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage. 2000;12:20–27.
    1. Larson R., Richards M.H. Daily companionship in late childhood and early adolescence: changing developmental contexts. Child Development. 1991;62:284–300.
    1. Legrand D., Ruby P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychological Review. 2009;116(1):252–282.
    1. Li S.C., Lindenberger U., Bäckman L. Dopaminergic modulation of cognition across development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:625–630.
    1. Lin A., Adolphs R., Rangel A. Social and monetary reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:274–281.
    1. Lombardo M.V., Chakrabarti B., Bullmore E.T., Wheelwright S.J., Sadek S.A., Suckling J., MRC AIMS Consortium, Baron-Cohen S. Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22(7):1623–1635.
    1. Lou H.C., Luber B., Crupain M., Keenan J.P., Nowak M., Kjae T.W., Sackeim H.A., Lisanby S.H. Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2004;101(17):6827–6832.
    1. Leube D.T., Knoblich G., Erb M., Kircher T.T.J. Observing one's hand become anarchic: an fMRI study of action identification. Consciousness and Cognition. 2003;12:597–608.
    1. Macrae C.N., Moran J.M., Heatherton T.F., Banfield J.F., Kelley W.M. Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:647–654.
    1. Marsh H.W. Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept: preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1989;81(3):417–430.
    1. Mason M.F., Dyer R., Norton M.I. Neural mechanisms of social influence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processesm. 2009;110
    1. Masten A.S., Coatsworth J.D., Neeman J., Gest S.D., Tellegen A., Garmezy N. The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adolescence. Child Development. 1995;66(6):1635–1659.
    1. McClure S.M., York M.K., Montague P.R. The neural substrates of reward processing in humans: the modern role of fMRI. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:260–268.
    1. Mitchell J.P., Banaji M.R., Macrae C.N. The link between social cognition and self-referential thought n the medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17(8):1306–1315.
    1. Montemayor R., Einsen M. The development of self-conceptions from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1977;13(4):314–319.
    1. Moore W.E., III, Merchant J.S., Kahn L.E., Pfeifer J.H. Like me?: ventromedial prefrontal cortex is sensitive to both personal relevance and self-similarity during social comparison. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013
    1. Moran J.M., Lee S.M., Gabrielli J.D.E. Dissociable neural systems supporting knowledge about human character and appearance in ourselves and others. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23(9):2222–2230.
    1. Morin A., Michaud J. Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: inner speech use during self-related processing. Brain Research Bulletin. 2007;74:387–396.
    1. Nakao T., Miyatani M., Nakao M., Takezawa T., Maruishi M., Mauranaka H., Dojo H. Does medial prefrontal cortex activity during self-knowledge reference reflect the uniqueness of self-knowledge? Japanese Psychological Research. 2009;51(2):69–84.
    1. Nelson E.E., Leinbenluft E., McClure E.B., Pine D.S. The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35:163–174.
    1. Northoff G., Hayes D.J. Is our self nothing but reward? Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69(11):1019–1025.
    1. Northoff G., Heinzel A., de Greck M., Bermpohl F., Dobrowolny H., Panksepp Self-referential processing in our brain: a meta-analysis of imaging studies of the self. Neuroimage. 2006;31:440–457.
    1. Northoff G., Qin P., Feinberg T.E. Brain imaging of the self-conceptual, anatomical, and methodological issues. Consciousness and Cognition. 2010;20(1):52–63.
    1. Nurmi J.-E. Socialization and self-development: channeling, selection, adjustment, and reflection. In: Lerner R.M., Steinberg L., editors. Handbook of Adolescence Psychology. 2nd ed. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ: 2004. pp. 85–124.
    1. Ochsner K.N., Beer J.S., Robertson E.R., Cooper J.C., Gabrieli J.D., Kihsltrom J.F. The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage. 2005;28(4):797–814.
    1. Petersen A., Crockett L., Richards M., Boxer A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1988;17(2):117–133.
    1. Petersen S.E., Dubis J.W. The mixed block/event-related design. Neuroimage. 2011;62(2):1177–1184.
    1. Pfeifer J.H., Lieberman M.D., Dapretto M. I know you are but what am I?: neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19(8):1323–1337.
    1. Pfeifer J.H., Masten C., Borofsky L.A., Dapretto M., Fuligni A.J., Lieberman M.D. Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective-taking informs self-perception. Child Development. 2009;80(4):1016–1038.
    1. Pfeifer J.H., Merchant J.S., Colich N.L., Hernandez L.M., Rudie J.D., Dapretto M. Neural and behavioral responses during self-evaluative processes differ in youth with and without autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(2):272–285.
    1. Pfeifer J.H., Kahn L.E., Merchant J.S., Peake S.A., Veroude K., Masten C.L., Lieberman M.D., Mazziotta J.C., Dapretto M. Longitudinal change in the neural bases of adolescent social self-evaluations: effects of age and pubertal development. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013
    1. Pfeifer J.H., Peake S.J. Self-development: integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;2:55–69.
    1. Phan K.L., Taylor S.F., Welsh R.C., Ho S.-H., Britton J.C., Liberzon I. Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2004;21:768–780.
    1. Powell L.J., Macrae C.N., Cloutier J., Metcalfe J., Mitchell J.P. Dissociable neural substrates of agentic versus conceptual representations of self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;22(10):2186–2197.
    1. Qin P., Northoff G. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? Neuroimage. 2011;57:1221–1233.
    1. Ray R.D., Shelton A.L., Hollon N.G., Michel B.D., Frankel C.B., Gross J.J., Gabrielli J.D.E. Cognitive and neural development of individuated self-representation in children. Child Development. 2009;80(4):1232–1242.
    1. Rogers R.D., Owen A.M., Middleton H.C., Williams E.J., Pickard J.D., Sahakian B.J., Robbins T.W. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;20(19):9029–9038.
    1. Rosenberg M. Basic Books; New York, NY: 1979. Conceiving the Self.
    1. Saxe R. Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2006;16:235–239.
    1. Schmitz T.W., Kawahara-Baccus T.N., Johnson S.C. Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2004;22:941–947.
    1. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80(1):1–27.
    1. Sebastian C.L., Fontaine N.M.G., Bird G., Blakemore S.-J., De Brito S.A., McCrory E.J.P., Viding E. Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7(1):53–63.
    1. Secord P., Peevers B. The development of social cognition. In: Mischel T., editor. Understanding Other Persons. Blackwell; Oxford, UK: 1974.
    1. Selman R. Academic Press; New York, NY: 1980. The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding.
    1. Shirk S.R., Renouf A.G. The tasks of self-development in middle childhood and early adolescence. In: Lipka R.P., Brinthaupt T.M., editors. Self-Perspectives Across the Lifespan. State University of New York Press; Albany, NY: 1992. pp. 53–89.
    1. Shrauger J.S., Schoeneman T.J. Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept: through the looking glass darkly. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;37:273–285.
    1. Steinberg L., Morris A.S. Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:83–110.
    1. Steinberg L., Silverberg S. The vicissitudes of autonomy in early adolescence. Child Development. 1986;57:841–851.
    1. Sussman S., Dent C.W., McAdams L.A., Stacy A.W., Burton D., Flay B.R. Group self-identification and adolescent cigarette smoking: a 1-year prospective study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103(3):576–580.
    1. Tacikowski P., Brechmann A., Marchewka A., Jednorog K., Dobrowolny M., Nowicka A. Is it about the self of the significance? An fMRI study of self-name recognition. Social Neuroscience. 2010;6(1):98–207.
    1. Tamir D.I., Mitchell J.P. Disclosing information about the self is intrinsically rewarding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(21):8038–8043.
    1. Tice D.M., Wallace H.M. The reflected self: creating yourself as, (you think) others see you. In: Leary M.R., Tangney J.P., editors. Handbook of Self and Identity. Guilford; New York, NY: 2003. pp. 91–105.
    1. Thesen S., Held O., Muller E., Scad L.R. Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image-based tracking for real-time fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;44(3):457–465.
    1. Van Leijenhorst L., Zanolie K., van Meel C.S., Westenberg P.M., Rombounts S.A.R.B., Crone E.A. What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:61–69.
    1. Vartanian L.R. Adolescents’ reactions to hypothetical peer group conversations: evidence for an imaginary audience? Adolescence. 2001;36(142):347–380.
    1. Volkow N.D., Wang W.J., Fowler J.S., Logan J., Jayne M., Francschi D., Wong C., Gatley S.J., Gifford A.N., Ding Y.-S., Pappas N. Nonhedonic food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect. Synapse. 2002;44:175–180.
    1. Wang A.T., Lee S.S., Sigman M., Dapretto M. Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2006;1(2):107–121.
    1. Whitfield-Gabrielli S., Moran J.M., Nieto-Castañón A., Triantafyllou C., Saxe R., Gabrieli J.D.E. Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2011;55:225–232.
    1. Zaki J., Schirmer J., Mitchell J.P. Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psychological Science. 2011;22:894–900.
    1. Zhu L., Guo X., Li J., Zheng L., Wang Q., Yang Z. Hippocampal activity is associated with self-descriptive memory, whereas self-reference effect in memory depends on medial prefrontal activity. Hippocampus. 2012;22(7):1540–1552.
    1. Zink C.F., Pagnoni G., Martin M.E., Dhamala M., Berns G.S. Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(22):8092–8097.

Source: PubMed

Подписаться