Similarity in Chronotype and Preferred Time for Sex and Its Role in Relationship Quality and Sexual Satisfaction

Paulina Jocz, Maciej Stolarski, Konrad S Jankowski, Paulina Jocz, Maciej Stolarski, Konrad S Jankowski

Abstract

Recently, the issue of diurnal preferences has been increasingly studied within the context of romantic relationships and sexual functioning. In the present paper we apply a dyadic design to investigate the role of romantic partners' diurnal preferences in determining a variety of relationship outcomes. A sample of 91 heterosexual couples completed a set of questionnaires measuring relationship satisfaction, sexual satisfaction, and morningness-eveningness, and answered questions regarding their actual and preferred time for sexual activity. Conducted analyses revealed that similarity in chronotype between partners and female morningness fostered relationship satisfaction in females, but not in males. Furthermore, morningness-eveningness was associated with preferred time for sex in males, but not in females, who in principle preferred evening hours. Although actual time for sex was up to the female preference, sexual satisfaction in both genders was associated with lower discrepancy in their preferred time for sex and greater frequency of intercourse. In sum, these results indicate that chronotype and time for sex are important factors affecting sexual and relationships satisfaction in heterosexual couples.

Keywords: chronotype; dyadic design; morningness-eveningness; relationship satisfaction; sexual satisfaction; time for sex.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Percentage of subjects in each chronotype group declaring a given time interval when they desired sex the most. Females (A) and males (B) are presented separately. Expected percentage assuming no circadian variation is 12.5 for each time interval.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Percentage of subjects in each chronotype group declaring a given time interval when they usually undertook sexual activity. Females (A) and males (B) are presented separately. Expected percentage assuming no circadian variation is 12.5 for each time interval.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Percentage of subjects in total declaring a given time interval when they desired sex the most and usually undertook sexual activity. Females (A) and males (B) are presented separately.

References

    1. Adan A., Archer S. N., Hidalgo M. P., Di Milia L., Natale V., Randler C. (2012). Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol. Int. 29, 1153–1175. 10.3109/07420528.2012.719971
    1. Bailey M., Silver R. (2014). Sex differences in circadian timing systems: implications for disease. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35, 111–139. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.11.003
    1. Bailey S. L., Heitkemper M. M. (2001). Circadian rhythmicity of cortisol and body temperature: morningness-eveningness effects. Chronobiol. Int. 18, 249–261. 10.1081/CBI-100103189
    1. Barak Y., Stein D., Ring A., Ticher A., Elizur A. (1997). Patterns of first intercourse: a survey among Israeli women. Biol. Rhythm Res. 28, 36–41. 10.1076/brhm.28.1.36.12972
    1. Baumeister R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: the female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychol. Bull. 126, 347–374. 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.347
    1. Baumeister R. F. (2004). Gender and erotic plasticity: sociocultural influences on the sex drive. Sex. Relash. Ther. 19, 133–139. 10.1080/14681990410001691343
    1. Berdynaj D., Boudissa S. N., Grieg M. S., Hope C., Mahamed S. H., Norbury R. (2016). Effect of chronotype on emotional processing and risk taking. Chronobiol. Int. 33, 406–418. 10.3109/07420528.2016.1146739
    1. Blackwell D., Lichter D. (2000). Mate selection among married and cohabiting couples. J. Fam. Issues. 21, 275–302. 10.1177/019251300021003001
    1. Buss D. M. (1984). Marital assortment for personality dispositions: assessment with three different data sources. Behav. Genet. 14, 111–123. 10.1007/BF01076408
    1. Buss D. M. (2003). The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating, Revised Edition. New York, NY: Basic Books.
    1. Call V., Sprecher S., Schwartz P. (1995). The incidence and frequency of marital sex in a national sample. J. Marriage Fam. 57, 639–652. 10.2307/353919
    1. Colom R., Aluja-Fabregat A., García-López Ó. (2002). Tendencias de emparejamiento selectivo en inteligencia, dureza de carácter, extraversión e inestabilidad emocional [Assortative mating in intelligence, psychoticism, extraversion, and neuroticism]. Psicothema 14, 154–158.
    1. Davies S., Katz J., Jackson J. L. (1999). Sexual desire discrepancies: effects on sexual and relationship satisfaction in heterosexual dating couples. Arch. Sex. Behav. 28, 553–567.
    1. Díaz-Morales J. F., Randler C. (2008). Morningness-eveningness among German and Spanish adolescents 12–18 years. Eur. Psychol. 13, 214–221. 10.1027/1016-9040.13.3.214
    1. Dieckmann U., Doebeli M. (1999). On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature 400, 354–357. 10.1038/22521
    1. Domanski H., Przybysz D. (2007). Homogamia Małzenska a Hierarchie Społeczne [Marital Homogamy and Social Hierarchies]. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo IFIS PAN.
    1. Escorial S., Martín-Buro C. (2012). The role of personality and intelligence in assortative mating. Span. J. Psychol. 15, 680–687. 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2012.v15.n2.38879
    1. Feng D., Baker L. (1994). Spouse similarity in attitudes, personality and psychological well-being. Behav. Genet. 24, 357–364. 10.1007/BF01067537
    1. Goldstein D., Hahn C. S., Hasher L., Wiprzycka U. J., Zelazo P. D. (2007). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: is there a synchrony effect? Pers. Indiv. Differ. 42, 431–440. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008
    1. Gonzaga G. C., Carter S., Buckwalter J. G. (2010). Assortative mating, satisfaction, and relationship satisfaction in married couples. Pers. Relationship 17, 634–644. 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01309.x
    1. Gunawardane K. G. C., Custance D. M., Piffer D. (2011). Evidence of sexual selection for evening orientation in human males: a cross cultural study in Italy and Sri Lanka. Interdisc. Bio. Central. 3:13 10.4051/ibc.2011.3.4.0013
    1. Hendrick S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. J. Marriage Fam. 50, 93–98. 10.2307/352430
    1. Hogben A. L., Ellis J., Archer S. N., von Schantz M. (2007). Conscientiousness is a predictor of diurnal preference. Chronobiol. Int. 24, 1249–1254. 10.1080/07420520701791596
    1. Hudson W. W., Harrison D. F., Crosscup P. C. (1981). A short-form scale to measure sexual discord in dyadic relationships. J. Sex. Res. 17, 157–174.
    1. Jankowski K. S. (2015a). Is the shift in chronotype associated with an alteration in well-being? Biol. Rhythm Res. 46, 237–248. 10.1080/09291016.2014.985000
    1. Jankowski K. S. (2015b). Composite scale of morningness: psychometric properties, validity with munich chronotype questionnaire and age/sex differences in Poland. Eur. Psychiatry. 30, 166–171. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.01.004
    1. Jankowski K. S. (2016). Morningness-eveningness and depressive symptoms: test on the components level with CES-D in Polish students. J. Affect. Disord. 196, 47–53. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.015
    1. Jankowski K. S., Díaz-Morales J. F., Randler C. (2014a). Chronotype, gender, and time for sex. Chronobiol. Int. 31, 911–916. 10.3109/07420528.2014.925470
    1. Jankowski K. S., Díaz-Morales J. F., Vollmer C., Randler C. (2014b). Morningness-eveningness and sociosexuality: evening females are less restricted than morning ones. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 68, 13–17. 10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.006
    1. Keller D. C., Thiessen D., Young R. K. (1996). Mate assortment in dating and married couples. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 21, 217–221. 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00066-9
    1. Lamkin J., Campbell W. K., Miller J. D. (2015). An exploration of the correlates of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism in romantic relationships: homophily, partner characteristics, and dyadic adjustment. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 79, 166–171. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.029
    1. Larson J. H., Crane D. R., Smith C. W. (1991). Morning and night couples: the effect of wake and sleep patterns on marital adjustment. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 17, 53–65. 10.1111/j.1752-0606.1991.tb00864.x
    1. Lawrance K. A., Byers E. S. (1995). Sexual satisfaction in long-term heterosexual relationships: the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction. Pers. Relationship. 2, 267–285. 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1995.tb00092.x
    1. Le B., Dove N. L., Agnew C. R., Korn M. S., Mutso A. A. (2010). Predicting nonmarital romantic relationship dissolution: a meta-analytic synthesis. Pers. Relationship. 17, 377–390. 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01285.x
    1. Leiblum S. R. (2002). Reconsidering gender differences in sexual desire: an update. Sex. Relatsh. Ther. 17, 57–68. 10.1080/14681990220108027
    1. Leonhard C., Randler C. (2009). In sync with the family: children and partners influence the sleep–wake circadian rhythm and social habits of women. Chronobiol. Int. 26, 510–525. 10.1080/07420520902821101
    1. Litzinger S., Gordon K. C. (2005). Exploring relationships among communication, sexual satisfaction, and marital satisfaction. J. Sex Marital Ther. 31, 409–424. 10.1080/00926230591006719
    1. Luo S., Klohnen E. C. (2005). Assortative mating and marital quality in newlyweds: a couple-centered approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 304–326. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.304
    1. Lykins A. D., Janssen E., Graham C. A. (2006). The relationship between negative mood and sexuality in heterosexual college women and men. J. Sex Res. 43, 136–143. 10.1080/00224490609552308
    1. Malouff J. M., Thorsteinsson E. B., Schutte N. S., Bhullar N., Rooke S. E. (2010). The five-factor model of personality and relationship satisfaction of intimate partners: a meta-analysis. J. Res. Pers. 44, 124–127. 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.004
    1. Mark K. P., Garcia J. R., Fisher H. E. (2015). Perceived emotional and sexual satisfaction across sexual relationship contexts: Gender and sexual orientation differences and similarities. Can. J. Hum. Sex. 24, 120–130. 10.3138/cjhs.242-A8
    1. May C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 142–147. 10.3758/BF03210822
    1. McCrae R. R., Martin T. A., Hrebícková M., Urbánek T., Boomsma D. I., Willemsen G. P. T., et al. (2008). Personality trait similarity between spouses in four cultures. J. Pers. 73, 1137–1164. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00517.x
    1. Molden D. C., Dweck C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: a lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. Am. Psychol. 61, 192–203. 10.1037/0003-066X.61.3.192
    1. Natale V., Cicogna P. C. (2002). Morningness-eveningness dimension: is it really a continuum? Pers. Indiv. Differ. 32, 809–816. 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00085-X
    1. Oginska H., Fafrowicz M., Golonka K., Marek T., Mojsa-Kaja J., Tucholska K. (2010). Chronotype, sleep loss, and diurnal pattern of salivary cortisol in a simulated daylong driving. Chronobiol. Int. 27, 959–974. 10.3109/07420528.2010.489412
    1. Palmer J. D., Udry J. R., Morris N. M. (1982). Diurnal and weekly, but no lunar rhythm in human copulation. Hum. Biol. 54, 111–121.
    1. Partonen T. (2015). Chronotype and health outcomes. Curr. Sleep. Med. Rep. 1, 205–211. 10.1007/s40675-015-0022-z
    1. Piffer D. (2010). Sleep patterns and sexual selection: an evolutionary approach. Mankind. Quart. 50, 361–375.
    1. Rammstedt B., Spinath F. M., Richter D., Schupp J. (2013). Partnership longevity and personality congruence in couples. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 54, 832–835. 10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.007
    1. Randler C. (2007). Gender differences in morningness-eveningness assessed by self-report questionnaires: a meta-analysis. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 43, 1667–1675. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.004
    1. Randler C., Barrenstein S., Vollmer C., Díaz-Morales J. F., Jankowski K. S. (2014). Women would like their partners to be more synchronized with them in their sleep-wake rhythm. Span. J. Psychol. 17:e70. 10.1017/sjp.2014.72
    1. Randler C., Ebenhöh N., Fischer A., Höchel S., Schroff C., Stoll J. C., et al. (2012a). Chronotype but not sleep length is related to salivary testosterone in young adult men. Psychoneuroendocrino 37, 1740–1744. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.008
    1. Randler C., Ebenhöh N., Fischer A., Höchel S., Schroff C., Stoll J. C., et al. (2012b). Eveningness is related to men's mating success. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 53, 263–267. 10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.025
    1. Randler C., Jankowski K. S., Rahafar A., Díaz-Morales J. F. (2016). Sociosexuality, morningness-eveningness, and sleep duration. SAGE Open 6, 1–8. 10.1177/2158244015621958
    1. Randler C., Kretz S. (2011). Assortative mating in morningness-eveningness. Int. J. Psychol. 46, 91–96. 10.1080/00207594.2010.518237
    1. Refinetti R. (2005). Time for sex: Nycthemeral distribution of human sexual behavior. J. Circ. Rhythms 3:4. 10.1186/1740-3391-3-4
    1. Roenneberg T., Kuehnle T., Pramstaller P. P., Ricken J., Havel M., Guth A., et al. . (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr. Biol. 14, R1038–R1039. 10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.039
    1. Smieja M., Stolarski M. (2018). Assortative mating for emotional intelligence. Curr. Psychol. 37:180. 10.1007/s12144-016-9501-8
    1. Smith C. S., Reilly C., Midkiff K. (1989). Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestion for an improved measure of morningness. J. Appl. Psychol. 75, 728–738. 10.1037/0021-9010.74.5.728
    1. Stolarski M., Jankowski K. S. (2015). Morningness-eveningness and performance-based emotional intelligence. Biol. Rhythm Res. 46, 417–423. 10.1080/09291016.2015.1020199
    1. Stolarski M., Postek S., Smieja M. (2011). Emotional intelligence and conflict resolution strategies in romantic heterosexual couples. Stud. Psychol. 49, 65–76. 10.2478/v10167-010-0041-9
    1. Stolarski M., Wojtkowska K., Kwiecinska M. (2016). Time for love: partners' time perspectives predict relationship satisfaction in romantic heterosexual couples. Time Soc. 25, 552–574. 10.1177/0961463X15596703
    1. Vandenberg S. G. (1972). Assortative mating, or who marries whom? Behav. Genet. 2, 127–157. 10.1007/BF01065686
    1. van Leeuwen M., van den Berg S. M., Boomsma D. I. (2008). A twin-family study of general IQ. Learn. Individ. Differ. 18, 76–88. 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.006
    1. Watson D., Klohnen E. C., Casillas A., Nus Simms E., Haig J., Berry D. S. (2004). Match makers and deal breakers: analyses of assortative mating in newlywed couples. J. Pers. 72, 1029–1068. 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00289.x
    1. Zeidner M., Kaluda I. (2008). Romantic love: What's emotional intelligence (EI) got to do with it? Pers. Indiv. Differ. 44, 1684–1695. 10.1016/j.paid.2008.01.018

Source: PubMed

Подписаться