Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: towards a multidimensional model

Franck Ramus, Chloe R Marshall, Stuart Rosen, Heather K J van der Lely, Franck Ramus, Chloe R Marshall, Stuart Rosen, Heather K J van der Lely

Abstract

An on-going debate surrounds the relationship between specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia, in particular with respect to their phonological abilities. Are these distinct disorders? To what extent do they overlap? Which cognitive and linguistic profiles correspond to specific language impairment, dyslexia and comorbid cases? At least three different models have been proposed: the severity model, the additional deficit model and the component model. We address this issue by comparing children with specific language impairment only, those with dyslexia-only, those with specific language impairment and dyslexia and those with no impairment, using a broad test battery of language skills. We find that specific language impairment and dyslexia do not always co-occur, and that some children with specific language impairment do not have a phonological deficit. Using factor analysis, we find that language abilities across the four groups of children have at least three independent sources of variance: one for non-phonological language skills and two for distinct sets of phonological abilities (which we term phonological skills versus phonological representations). Furthermore, children with specific language impairment and dyslexia show partly distinct profiles of phonological deficit along these two dimensions. We conclude that a multiple-component model of language abilities best explains the relationship between specific language impairment and dyslexia and the different profiles of impairment that are observed.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Three models of the relationship between SLI and dyslexia, according to performance along non-phonological and phonological language skills. (A) Severity model, (B) additional deficit model and (C) component model.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean and standard error of theory-driven components for each group. The circle highlights an interaction of interest between group and factor.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Distribution of individual children according to their phonological skills and non-phonological language skills. Lines correspond to a −1.5-SD threshold.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Distribution of individual children according to their phonological skills and phonological representations [two outliers with SLI and dyslexia (SLI + dyslexia) are out of the range of this graph along the y-axis]. Lines correspond to a −1.5-SD threshold.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Models of SLI and dyslexia in three and in four dimensions. (A) The two-dimensions proposed by Bishop and Snowling (2004). (B) The two phonological dimensions evidenced in the present study. (C) A hypothetical split of the non-phonological dimension into two distinct ones: lexical and grammatical skills.

References

    1. Ahissar M. Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:458–65.
    1. Bailey C, Manis F, Pederson W, Seidenberg MS. Variation amongst developmental dyslexics: evidence from a printed-word-learning task. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:125–54.
    1. Baker RJ, Rosen S. Evaluation of maximum-likelihood threshold estimation with tone-in-noise masking. Br J Audiol. 2001;35:43–52.
    1. Berent I, Vaknin-Nusbaum V, Balaban E, Galaburda AM. Dyslexia impairs speech recognition but can spare phonological competence. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e44875.
    1. Bishop DVM. Test for reception of grammar. London: Harcourt Assessment, Psychological Corporation; 2003.
    1. Bishop DVM, McDonald D, Bird S, Hayiou-Thomas ME. Children who read words accurately despite language impairment: who are they and how do they do it? Child Dev. 2009;80:593–605.
    1. Bishop DVM, North T, Donlan C. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37:391–403.
    1. Bishop DVM, Snowling MJ. Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? Psychol Bull. 2004;130:858–86.
    1. Bosse ML, Tainturier MJ, Valdois S. Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition. 2007;104:198–230.
    1. Bradley L, Bryant PE. Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature. 1978;271:746–747.
    1. Bradley L, Bryant PE. Categorising sounds and learning to read—a causal connection. Nature. 1983;301:419–421.
    1. Bryant FB, Yarnold PR. Principal components analysis and exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. In: Grimm LG, Yarnold PR, editors. Reading and understanding multivariate analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1995. pp. 99–136.
    1. Catts HW, Adlof SM, Hogan TP, Weismer SE. Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48:1378–96.
    1. Conti-Ramsden G, Betting N, Simkin Z, Knox E. Follow-up of children attending infant language units: outcomes at 11 years of age. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2001;36:207–19.
    1. Dale PS, Harlaar N, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Plomin R. The etiology of diverse receptive language skills at 12 years. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53:982–92.
    1. de Bree E, Kerkhoff A. Bempen or bemben: differences between children at-risk of dyslexia and children with SLI on a morpho-phonological task. Sci Stud Read. 2010;14:85–109.
    1. Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N): dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia. 1976;14:471–9.
    1. Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Whetton C, Burley J. British picture vocabulary scales. 2nd edn. Windsor: NFER Nelson; 1997.
    1. Ebbels SH, Dockrell JE, van der Lely HK. Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment (SLI) Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47:257–73.
    1. Elliott CD. British ability scales. 2nd edn. Windsor: NFER Nelson; 1996.
    1. Fonteneau E, van der Lely HK. Electrical brain responses in language-impaired children reveal grammar-specific deficits. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e1832.
    1. Frederickson N, Frith U, Reason R. Phonological assessment battery. Windsor: NFER-NELSON; 1997.
    1. Frith U. Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In: Patterson KE, Marshall JC, Coltheart M, editors. Surface dyslexia. London: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1985. pp. 301–30.
    1. Gallon N, Harris J, van der Lely HK. Non-word repetition: an investigation of phonological complexity in children with Grammatical-SLI. Clin Linguist Phon. 2007;21:435–55.
    1. Gardner H, Froud K, McClelland A, Van der Lely HK. The development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language difficulties in young children. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2006;41:531–40.
    1. Gathercole S, Baddeley A. Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connection? J Mem Lang. 1990;29:336–60.
    1. Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. The children's test of nonword repetition. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1996.
    1. German DJ. Test of word finding. 2nd edn. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed; 2000.
    1. Hatcher L. A step-by-step approach to using the SAS system for factor analysis and structural equation modeling. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 1994.
    1. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Kovas Y, Harlaar N, Plomin R, Bishop DV, Dale PS. Common aetiology for diverse language skills in 4 1/2-year-old twins. J Child Lang. 2006;33:339–68.
    1. Hazan V, Messaoud-Galusi S, Rosen S, Nouwens S, Shakespeare B. Speech perception abilities of adults with dyslexia: is there any evidence for a true deficit? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52:1510–29.
    1. Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:393–402.
    1. Inoue T, Higashibara F, Okazaki S, Maekawa H. Speech perception in noise deficits in Japanese children with reading difficulties: effects of presentation rate. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32:2748–57.
    1. Joanisse M, Manis F, Keating P, Seidenberg MS. Language deficits in dyslexic children: speech perception, phonology and morphology. J Exp Child Psychol. 2000;77:30–60.
    1. Kamhi A, Catts H. Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1986;51:337–47.
    1. Kelso K, Fletcher J, Lee P. Reading comprehension in children with specific language impairment: an examination of two subgroups. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:39–57.
    1. Leonard L. Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998.
    1. Loeb DF, Leonard LB. Subject case marking and verb morphology in normally developing and specifically language-impaired children. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34:340–6.
    1. Marshall CR, Harcourt-Brown S, Ramus F, van der Lely HK. The link between prosody and language skills in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and/or dyslexia. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44:466–88.
    1. Marshall CR, Ramus F, van der Lely H. Do children with dyslexia and/or specific language impairment compensate for place assimilation? Insight into phonological grammar and representations. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2011;27:563–86.
    1. Marshall CR, van der Lely HKJ. Effects of word position and stress on onset cluster production: evidence from typical development, specific language impairment, and dyslexia. Language. 2009;85:39–57.
    1. McArthur GM, Hogben JH, Edwards VT, Heath SM, Mengler ED. On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:869–74.
    1. Messaoud-Galusi S, Marshall CR. Introduction to this special issue. Exploring the overlap between dyslexia and SLI: the role of phonology. Sci Stud Read. 2010;14:1–7.
    1. Montgomery JW. Examination of phonological working memory in specifcally language-impaired children. Appl Psycholinguist. 1995;16:335–78.
    1. Mundy IR, Carroll JM. Speech prosody and developmental dyslexia: reduced phonological awareness in the context of intact phonological representations. J Cogn Psychol. 2012;24:560–81.
    1. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ, Dean P. Dyslexia, development and the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:515–16.
    1. Oetting JB, Horohov JE. Past-tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40:62–74.
    1. Pennington BF, Bishop DV. Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:283–306.
    1. Ramus F. Outstanding questions about phonological processing in dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7:197–216.
    1. Ramus F. Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:212–18.
    1. Ramus F. Neurobiology of dyslexia: a reinterpretation of the data. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:720–6.
    1. Ramus F, Ahissar M. Developmental dyslexia: the difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2012;29:104–22.
    1. Ramus F, Szenkovits G. What phonological deficit? Q J Exp Psychol. 2008;61:129–41.
    1. Raven JC. Raven’s progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press, Ltd.; 1998.
    1. Rice ML, Wexler K. Towards tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. J Speech Hear Res. 1996;39:1239–57.
    1. Rispens J, Been P. Subject-verb agreement and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment (SLI): a closer look. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:293–305.
    1. Robertson EK, Joanisse MF. Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: the roles of syntax and working memory. Appl Psycholinguist. 2010;31:141–65.
    1. Rosen S. Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? J Phon. 2003;31:509–27.
    1. Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Müller H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: test of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol Res Online. 2003;8:23–74.
    1. Semel E, Wiig E, Secord W. Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals. San Antonio, CA: Psychological Corporation; 1995.
    1. Snowling MJ. Dyslexia. 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell; 2000.
    1. Soroli E, Szenkovits G, Ramus F. Exploring dyslexics' phonological deficit III: foreign speech perception and production. Dyslexia. 2010;16:318–40.
    1. Stark RE, Tallal P, McCauley RJ. Language, speech, and reading disorders in children : neuropsychological studies. Boston, MA: Little, Brown; 1988.
    1. Stein JF. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7:12–36.
    1. Szenkovits G, Ramus F. Exploring dyslexics' phonological deficit I: lexical versus sub-lexical and input versus output processes. Dyslexia. 2005;11:253–68.
    1. Tallal P. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain Lang. 1980;9:182–98.
    1. Tallal P. Language learning disabilities: integrating research approaches. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2003;12:206–11.
    1. Tallal P, Piercy M. Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature. 1973;241:468–9.
    1. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M. The prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40:1245–60.
    1. van der Lely HK. Test of active and passive sentences. 1996. , London.
    1. van der Lely HK. Advanced-syntactic test of pronominal reference (A-STOP) 1997. , London.
    1. van der Lely HK. Verb agreement and tense test. 2000. , London.
    1. van der Lely HK. Domain-specific cognitive systems: insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:53–9.
    1. van der Lely HK, Howard D. Children with specific language impairment: linguistic impairment or short-term memory deficit? J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36:1193–207.
    1. van der Lely HK, Marshall CR. Assessing component language deficits in the early detection of reading difficulty risk. J Learn Disabil. 2010;43:357–68.
    1. van der Lely HK, Marshall CR. Grammatical-specific language impairment: a window onto domain specificity. In: Guendouzi J, Loncke F, Williams M, editors. Handbook of psycholinguistics and cog processing perspectives in communication disorders. Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press; 2011. pp. 403–19.
    1. van der Lely HK, Ullman M. Past tense morphology in SLI children and normally developing children. Lang Cogn Process. 2001;16:177–217.
    1. Vellutino FR. Dyslexia: research and theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1979.
    1. Vidyasagar TR, Pammer K. Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:57–63.
    1. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol Bull. 1987;101:192–212.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler objective reading dimensions. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1990.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1992.
    1. Wolf M, Bowers P. The 'double deficit hypothesis' for developmental dyslexia. J Educ Psychol. 1999;91:1–24.

Source: PubMed

Подписаться