Resting-state slow wave power, healthy aging and cognitive performance

Eleni L Vlahou, Franka Thurm, Iris-Tatjana Kolassa, Winfried Schlee, Eleni L Vlahou, Franka Thurm, Iris-Tatjana Kolassa, Winfried Schlee

Abstract

Cognitive functions and spontaneous neural activity show significant changes over the life-span, but the interrelations between age, cognition and resting-state brain oscillations are not well understood. Here, we assessed performance on the Trail Making Test and resting-state magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings from 53 healthy adults (18-89 years old) to investigate associations between age-dependent changes in spontaneous oscillatory activity and cognitive performance. Results show that healthy aging is accompanied by a marked and linear decrease of resting-state activity in the slow frequency range (0.5-6.5 Hz). The effects of slow wave power on cognitive performance were expressed as interactions with age: For older (>54 years), but not younger participants, enhanced delta and theta power in temporal and central regions was positively associated with perceptual speed and executive functioning. Consistent with previous work, these findings substantiate further the important role of slow wave oscillations in neurocognitive function during healthy aging.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(a) Topographic representation of the scalp distribution of Pearson product moment correlations between age and MEG power for slow-waves (0.5–6.5 Hz). Blue regions indicate negative correlations, red regions indicate positive correlations. The cluster of sensors showing significant linear decrease in MEG power with age (p = .009) is marked with white dots. (b) Scatterplot depicting linear correlations between cluster-averaged spectral power and chronological age (r = −.58, p < .001).
Figure 2. Scaled spectral power averaged over…
Figure 2. Scaled spectral power averaged over all sensors from the slow wave cluster.
Significant frequencies are shaded in gray. Participants were divided into a “younger” (n = 26, blue line) and an “older” (n = 27, red line) group by median split. Older participants exhibit reduced spectral power compared to younger for slow wave frequencies. The small peak at 16.6 Hz represents technical noise resulting from a railway system that operates in a distance of approximately 1 km to the lab.
Figure 3
Figure 3
(a) Illustration of the position of the selected sensor clusters for the five defined regions (Frontal, Central, Left Temporal, Right Temporal and Occipital). (b–d) Scatterplots depicting linear correlations between slow wave activity in temporal and central regions and performance (completion time) on the Trail Making Test, separately for “younger” (n = 26) and “older” (n = 27) participants (based on a median split). (b) Associations between performance on TMT-A and delta power (0.5–4 Hz) in the right temporal region. (c) Associations between performance on TMT-B and delta power in the central region. (d) Associations between performance on TMT-B and theta activity in the left temporal region.

References

    1. Hedden T. & Gabrieli J. D. E. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 5, 87–96 (2004).
    1. Salthouse T. A. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16, 754–760 (2010).
    1. Maddox T. W., Chandrasekaran B., Smayda K. & Yi H.-G. Dual systems of speech category learning across the lifespan. Psychol Aging 28, 1042–1056 (2013).
    1. Adank P. & Janse E. Comprehension of a novel accent by young and older listeners. Psychol Aging 25, 736–740 (2010).
    1. Salthouse T. A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 30, 507–514 (2009).
    1. Schaie K. W., Willis S. L. & Caskie G. I. The Seattle longitudinal study: relationship between personality and cognition. Aging, Neuropsychol C 11, 304–324 (2004).
    1. Nyberg L., Lövdén M., Riklund K., Lindenberger U. & Bäckman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn Sci 16, 292–305 (2012).
    1. Bartzokis G. et al. Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58, 461–465 (2001).
    1. Bäckman L., Lindenberger U., Li S.-C. & Nyberg L. Linking cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter functioning: Recent data and future avenues. Neurosci Biobehav R 34, 670–677 (2010).
    1. Shing Y. L. et al. Hippocampal subfield volumes: age, vascular risk, and correlation with associative memory. Front Aging Neurosci 3, (2011).
    1. Small S. A., Schobel S. A., Buxton R. B., Witter M. P. & Barnes C. A. A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 12, 585–601 (2011).
    1. Salthouse T. A. Neuroanatomical substrates of age-related cognitive decline. Psychol Bul 135, 753–784 (2011).
    1. Clark C. et al. Spontaneous alpha peak frequency predicts working memory performance across the age span. Int J Psychophysiol 53, 1–9 (2004).
    1. Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Rev 29, 169–195 (1999).
    1. Rossini P. M., Rossi S., Babiloni C. & Polich J. Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: From normal aging to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol 83, 375–400 (2007).
    1. McEvoy L. K., Pellouchoud E., Smith M. E. & Gevins A. Neurophysiological signals of working memory in healthy aging. Cognitive Brain Res 11, 363–376 (2001).
    1. Babiloni C. et al. Sources of cortical rhythms in adults during physiological aging: a multicentric EEG study. Hum Brain Mapp 27, 162–172 (2006).
    1. Leirer V. M. et al. Changes in cortical slow wave activity in healthy aging. Brain Imag Behav 5, 222–228 (2011).
    1. Finnigan S. & Robertson I. H. Resting EEG theta power correlates with cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Psychophysiol 48, 1083–1087 (2011).
    1. Cummins T. D. & Finnigan S. Theta power is reduced in cognitive aging. Int J Psychophysiol 66, 10–17 (2007).
    1. Hartikainen P., Soininen H., Partanen J., Helkala E. L. & Riekkinen P. Aging and spectral analysis of EEG in normal subjects: a link to memory and CSF AChE. Acta Neurol Scand 86, 148–55 (1992).
    1. Ho M.-C. et al. Task-related brain oscillations in normal aging. Health 4, 762–768 (2012).
    1. Stomrud E. et al. Slowing of EEG correlates with CSF biomarkers and reduced cognitive speed in elderly with normal cognition over 4 years. Neurobiol Aging 31, 215–223 (2008).
    1. Puligheddu et al. Age distribution of MEG spontaneous theta activity in healthy subjects. Brain Topogr 17, 165–175 (2005).
    1. Ward L. M. Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn Sci 7, 553–559 (2003).
    1. Engel A. K. & Fries P. Beta-band oscillations – signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol 20, 156–165 (2010).
    1. Kahana M. J., Seelig D. & Madsen J. R. Theta returns. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11, 739–744 (2001).
    1. Bosseler A. N. et al. Theta brain rhythms index perceptual narrowing in infant speech perception. Front Psychol 4, 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00690 (2013).
    1. Giraud A. L. & Poeppel D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat Neurosci, 15, 511–517 (2012).
    1. Schlee W. et al. Development of large-scale functional networks over the lifespan. Neurobiol Aging 33, 2411–2421 (2012).
    1. Greicius M. D., Krasnow B., Reiss A. L. & Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. PNAS 100, 253–258 (2003).
    1. Raichle M. E. et al. A default mode of brain function. PNAS 98, 676–682 (2001).
    1. Fernández A. et al. Focal temporoparietal slow activity in Alzheimer's Disease revealed by magnetencephalography. Biol Psychiatry 52, 764–770 (2002).
    1. Prichep L. S. et al. Prediction of longitudinal cognitive decline in normal elderly with subjective complaints using electrophysiological imaging. Neurobiol Aging 27, 471–481 (2006).
    1. Grunwald M. et al. Correlation between cortical θ activity and hippocampal volumes in health, mild cognitive impairment, and mild dementia. J Clin Neurophysiol 18, 178–184 (2001).
    1. Grunwald M., Hensel A., Wolf H., Weiss T. & Gertz H.-J. Does the hippocampal atrophy correlate with the cortical theta power in elderly subjects with a range of cognitive impairment? J Clin Neurophysiol 24, 22–26 (2007).
    1. Tombaugh T. N. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19, 203–214 (2004).
    1. Sánchez-Cubillo I. et al. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: Role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsych Soc 15, 438–450 (2009).
    1. Kouijzer M. E. J., de Moor J. M. H., Gerrits B. J. L., Congedo M. & van Schie H. T. Neurofeedback improves executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spect Dis 3, 145–162 (2009).
    1. Mathewson K. E. et al. Different slopes for different folks: Alpha and delta EEG power predict subsequent video game learning rate and improvements in cognitive control tasks. Psychophysiology 49, 1558–1570 (2012).
    1. Anguera J. A. et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101 (2013).
    1. Nigbur R., Ivanova G. & Stürmer B. Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference. Clin Neurophysiol 122, 2185–2194 (2011).
    1. Raghavachari S. et al. Gating of human theta oscillations by a working memory task. J Neurosci 21, 3175–3183 (2001).
    1. Caplan J. B. et al. Human θ oscillations related to sensorimotor integration and spatial learning. J Neurosci 23, 4726–4736 (2003).
    1. Anderson K. L., Rajagovindan R., Ghacibeh G. A., Meador K. J. & Ding M. Theta oscillations mediate interaction between prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe in human memory. Cereb Cortex 20, 1604–1612 (2010).
    1. Akaike H. [Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle]. Second International Symposium on Information Theory [Petrov, B. N. & Csaki, F. (eds.)] [267–281] (Akademiai Kiado, Budapest, 1973).
    1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) (Washington, DC, 2000).
    1. Ackenheil M., Stotz G., Dietz-Bauer R. & Vossen A. I. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview. German version 5.0.0. (Munich, 1999).
    1. Oldfield R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113 (1971).
    1. Morris J. C., Mohs R. C., Rogers H., Fillenbaum G. & Heyman A. Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Psychopharmacol Bull 24, 641–652 (1988).
    1. Oostenveld R., Fries P., Maris E. & Schoffelen J. M. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci (2011).
    1. Delorme A. & Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Meth 134, 9–21 (2004).
    1. Knösche T. R. Transformation of whole-head MEG recordings between different sensor positions. Biomedizinische Technik 47, 59–62 (2002).
    1. Maris E. & Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG and MEG-data. J Neurosci Meth 164, 177–190 (2007).
    1. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat 6, 65–70 (1979).

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe