A randomised active-controlled trial to examine the effects of an online mindfulness intervention on executive control, critical thinking and key thinking dispositions in a university student sample

Chris Noone, Michael J Hogan, Chris Noone, Michael J Hogan

Abstract

Background: Arguments for including mindfulness instruction in higher education have included claims about the benefits of mindfulness practice for critical thinking. While there is theoretical support for this claim, empirical support is limited. The aim of this study was to test this claim by investigating the effects of an online mindfulness intervention on executive function, critical thinking skills and associated thinking dispositions.

Method: Participants recruited from a university were randomly allocated, following screening, to either a mindfulness meditation group or a sham meditation group. Both the researchers and the participants were blind to group allocation. The intervention content for both groups was delivered through the Headspace online application, an application which provides guided meditations to users. Both groups were requested to complete 30 guided mindfulness meditation sessions across a 6 week period. Primary outcome measures assessed mindfulness, executive functioning, critical thinking, actively open-minded thinking and need for cognition. Secondary outcome measures assessed wellbeing, positive and negative affect, and real-world outcomes.

Results: In a series of full-information maximum likelihood analyses, significant increases in mindfulness dispositions and critical thinking scores were observed in both the mindfulness meditation and sham meditation groups. However, no significant effects of group allocation were observed for either primary or secondary measures. Furthermore, mediation analyses testing the indirect effect of group allocation through executive functioning performance did not reveal a significant result and moderation analyses showed that the effect of the intervention did not depend on baseline levels of the key thinking dispositions, actively open-minded thinking and need for cognition.

Conclusion: No evidence was found to suggest that engaging in guided mindfulness practice for 6 weeks using the online intervention method applied in this study improves critical thinking performance. While further research is warranted, claims regarding the benefits of mindfulness practice for critical thinking should be tempered in the meantime.

Trial registration: The study was initially registered in the AEA Social Science Registry before the recruitment was initiated (RCT ID: AEARCTR-0000756; 14/11/2015) and retrospectively registered in the ISRCTN registry ( RCT ID: ISRCTN16588423 ) in line with requirements for publishing the study protocol.

Conflict of interest statement

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study received full approval from the National University of Ireland, Galway Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was sought from all participants for both their participation and the publication of the results of the research. Participants were reminded of their freedom to withdraw at any time and that their data would be stored securely and anonymously. We had ethical approval to blind participants to their allocated condition and to inform participants that the trial involved comparison of two different forms of meditation rather than fully inform regarding the comparison of mindfulness meditation and a sham meditation at the outset. Full debriefing was carried out following the end of the follow-up data collection. The study was registered in the AEA Social Science Registry before the study was initiated (RCT ID: AEARCTR-0000756) and was registered in the ISRCTN registry following the start of the study to comply with requirements for publishing the study protocol (RCT ID: ISRCTN16588423; [61]).

Consent for publication

No individual data is presented in this article.

Competing interests

This intervention employed materials developed by Headspace Inc. and involved the use of their website and applications. Headspace had no control over the data or input into the design, analysis or reporting of the study but do retain the right to see the results of data analysis. The authors have no other competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
CONSORT [105] flowchart of progress through phases of the current study. Sample size (Incl. flow chart)

References

    1. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro SL, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004;11:230–41. 10.1093/clipsy/bph077.
    1. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Astin JA. Toward the integration of meditation into higher education: a review of research. Teach Coll Rec. 2011;113:493–528.
    1. Noone C, Bunting B, Hogan MJ, et al. Front Psychol. 2016;6:2043. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02043.
    1. Brendel D. There are risks to mindfulness at work. Harv Bus Rev. 2015;
    1. West RF, Toplak ME, Stanovich KE. Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. J Educ Psychol. 2008;100:930–941. doi: 10.1037/a0012842.
    1. Halpern DF. Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains. Am Psychol. 1998;53:449–455. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449.
    1. Halpern DF. The Nature and Nurture of Critical Thinking. Crit Think Psychol. 2007;c:1–15.
    1. Ku KYL, Ho IT. Metacognitive strategies that enhance critical thinking. Metacognition Learn. 2010;5:251–67. 10.1007/s11409-010-9060-6.
    1. Kuhn D. Metacognitive Development. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2000;9:178–81. 10.1111/1467-8721.00088.
    1. Heijltjes A, van Gog T, Leppink J, Paas F. Improving critical thinking: effects of dispositions and instructions on economics students’ reasoning skills. Learn Instr. 2014;29:31–42. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003.
    1. Larue O, Poirier P, Nkambou R. Hypothetical-thinking based on cognitive decoupling and thinking dispositions in a dual cognitive agent. Biol Inspired Cogn Archit. 2013;6:67–75. 10.1016/j.bica.2013.06.004.
    1. Ku KYL, Ho IT. Dispositional factors predicting Chinese students’ critical thinking performance. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;48:54–8. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.015.
    1. Magno C. The role of metacognitive skills in developing critical thinking. Metacognition Learn. 2010;5:137–56. 10.1007/s11409-010-9054-4.
    1. Hogan MJ, Dwyer CP, Harney OM, Noone C, Conway RJ. Metacognitive skill development and applied systems science: a framework of metacognitive skills, self-regulatory functions and real-world applications. 2015.
    1. Butler HA, Dwyer CP, Hogan MJ, Franco A, Rivas SF, Saiz C, et al. The Halpern critical thinking assessment and real-world outcomes: cross-national applications. Think Ski Creat. 2012;7:112–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2012.04.001.
    1. Butler HA. Halpern critical thinking assessment predicts real-world outcomes of critical thinking. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2012;26:721–9. 10.1002/acp.2851.
    1. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000.
    1. Hastie R, Dawes RM. Rational Choice in an Uncertain World: The Psychology of Judgement and Decision Making: SAGE Publications; 2010. p. 392.
    1. Myers DG. Intuition: its powers and perils: Yale University Press; 2004.
    1. Hilton DJ. Psychology and the financial markets: applications to understanding and remedying irrational decision-making. Psychol Econ Decis. 2003;1:273–297.
    1. Sunstein CR. Moral heuristics. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:531–541.
    1. Baron J, Bazerman MH, Shonk K. Enlarging the societal pie through wise legislation: a psychological perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2006;1:123–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00009.x.
    1. Reyna VF, Farley F. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol Sci public Interes. 2006;7:1–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x.
    1. Reyna VF, Lloyd FJ. Physician decision making and cardiac risk: effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2006;12:179. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.12.3.179.
    1. Gallant SN. Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: breaking down the benefit. Conscious Cogn. 2016;40:116–30. 10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.005.
    1. Ostafin BD, Kassman KT, Wessel I. Breaking the cycle of desire: mindfulness and executive control weaken the relation between an implicit measure of alcohol valence and preoccupation with alcohol-related thoughts. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):1153. doi: 10.1037/a0032621.
    1. Colzato LS, Ozturk A, Hommel B. Meditate to create: the impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Front Psychol. 2012;3:116. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00116.
    1. Ruedy NE, Schweitzer ME. In the Moment: The Effect of Mindfulness on Ethical Decision Making; 2011. 10.1007/s10551-011-0796-y.
    1. Shapiro SL, Jazaieri H, Goldin PR. Mindfulness-based stress reduction effects on moral reasoning and decision making. J Posit Psychol. 2012;7:504–515. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.723732.
    1. Hafenbrack AC, Kinias Z, Barsade SG. Debiasing the mind through meditation: mindfulness and the sunk-cost Bias. Psychol Sci. 2013. 10.1177/0956797613503853.
    1. Hart R, Ivtzan I, Hart D. Mind the gap in mindfulness research: a comparative account of the leading schools of thought. Rev Gen Psychol. 2013;17:453–66. 10.1037/a0035212.
    1. Evans JSBT. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:255–78. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629.
    1. Stanovich KE, Toplak ME. Defining features versus incidental correlates of type 1 and type 2 processing. Mind Soc. 2012;11:3–13. 10.1007/s11299-011-0093-6.
    1. Evans JSB, Stanovich KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:223–241. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685.
    1. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley A. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:174–80. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006.
    1. Toplak ME, Sorge GB, Benoit A, West RF, Stanovich KE. Decision-making and cognitive abilities: a review of associations between Iowa gambling task performance, executive functions, and intelligence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:562–81. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.002.
    1. Liberali JM, Reyna VF, Furlan S, Stein LM, Pardo ST. Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. J Behav Decis Mak. 2012;25:361–381. doi: 10.1002/bdm.752.
    1. Stanovich KE, West RF. On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;94:672–95. 10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672.
    1. Böckenholt U. The cognitive-miser response model: testing for intuitive and deliberate reasoning. Psychometrika. 2012;77:388–399. doi: 10.1007/s11336-012-9251-y.
    1. Campitelli G, Gerrans P. Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach. Mem Cogn. 2014;42:434–47. 10.3758/s13421-013-0367-9.
    1. Travers E, Rolison JJ, Feeney A. The time course of conflict on the cognitive reflection test. Cognition. 2016;150:109–18. 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.015.
    1. Chiesa A, Calati R, Serretti A. Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:449–64. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003.
    1. Teper R, Segal ZV, Inzlicht M. Inside the mindful mind: how mindfulness enhances emotion regulation through improvements in executive control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22:449–54. 10.1177/0963721413495869.
    1. de Neys W. Bias and conflict: a case for logical intuitions. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7:28–38.
    1. Thompson VA, Johnson SC. Conflict, metacognition, and analytic thinking. Think Reason. 2014;20:215–44.
    1. Pennycook G, Fugelsang JA, Koehler DJ. What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cogn Psychol. 2015;80(September 2016):34–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.05.001.
    1. Ostafin BD, Kassman KT. Stepping out of history: mindfulness improves insight problem solving. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21:1031–6. 10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.014.
    1. Greenberg J, Reiner K, Meiran N. “Mind the trap”: mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS One. 2012;7:e36206. 10.1371/journal.pone.0036206.
    1. Howells A, Ivtzan I, Eiroa-Orosa FJ. Putting the “app” in happiness: a randomised controlled trial of a smartphone-based mindfulness intervention to enhance wellbeing. J Happiness Stud. 2014. 10.1007/s10902-014-9589-1.
    1. Bostock SK, Steptoe A. Can finding headspace reduce work stress? A randomised controlled workplace trial of a mindfulness meditation app. Psychosom Med. 2013;75:A36–7.
    1. Ly KH, Trüschel A, Jarl L, Magnusson S, Windahl T, Johansson R, et al. Behavioural activation versus mindfulness-based guided self-help treatment administered through a smartphone application: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2014;4:e003440. 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003440.
    1. Lim D, Condon P, Desteno D. Mindfulness and Compassion: An Examination of Mechanism and Scalability; 2015. p. 1–8.
    1. Spijkerman MPJ, Pots WTM, Bohlmeijer ET. Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions in improving mental health: a review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;45:102–14. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.03.009.
    1. Quaglia JT, Braun SE, Freeman SP, Mcdaniel MA, Brown KW, Quaglia JT, et al. Meta-analytic evidence for effects of mindfulness training on dimensions of self-reported dispositional mindfulness meta-analytic evidence for effects of mindfulness training on dimensions of self-reported dispositional Mindfulnes. Psychol Assess. 2016;28:803–18.
    1. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Diamond BJ, David Z, Goolkasian P. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Conscious Cogn. 2010;19:597–605. 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014.
    1. Schmidt S, Grossman P, Schwarzer B, Jena S, Naumann J, Walach H. Treating fibromyalgia with mindfulness-based stress reduction: results from a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Pain. 2011;152. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.043.
    1. Zeidan F, Emerson NM, Farris SR, Ray JN, Jung Y, McHaffie JG, et al. Mindfulness meditation-based pain relief employs different neural mechanisms than placebo and sham mindfulness meditation-induced analgesia. J Neurosci. 2015;35:15307–25. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2542-15.2015.
    1. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Gordon NS, Goolkasian P. Effects of brief and sham mindfulness meditation on mood and cardiovascular variables. J Altern Complement Med. 2010;16:867–73. 10.1089/acm.2009.0321.
    1. Noone C, Hogan MJ. Improvements in Critical Thinking Performance Following Guided Mindfulness Meditation Depend on Need for Cognition and Open-mindedness. 2016.
    1. Lilley R, Whitehead M, Jones R, Pykett J. Mindfulness Behaviour Change and Engagement in Public Policy An Evaluation October 2014. 2014.
    1. Noone C, Hogan MJ. A protocol for a randomised active-controlled trial to evaluate the effects of an online mindfulness intervention on executive control, critical thinking and key thinking dispositions in a university student sample. BMC Psychol. 2016;4:1–12. 10.1186/s40359-016-0122-7.
    1. Boutron I, Moher D, Altman DG, Schulz KF, Ravaud P. Extending the CONSORT statement to randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatment: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2008;148:295–309. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-148-4-200802190-00008.
    1. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.
    1. Rossi JS. Statistical power analysis. In: Schinka WA, Velicer WF, editors. Handbook of psychology volume 2: research methods in psychology. Hoboken: Wiley; 2012. pp. 71–109.
    1. OASAS . Screening for co-occurring disorders using the Modified MINI Screen (MMS) Albany: OASAS; 2005.
    1. Sealed Envelope Ltd. Create a blocked randomisation list. 2015. . Accessed 2 Dec 2015.
    1. Draine SC. Inquisit 4. Seattle: Millisecond Software; 2014.
    1. G-JY P. The alpha and the omega of scale reliability and validity. Eur Health Psychol. 2014;16:56–69.
    1. Eisinga R, Te GM, Pelzer B. The reliability of a two-item scale: Pearson, Cronbach, or spearman-Brown? Int J Public Health. 2013;58:637–642. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0416-3.
    1. Halpern DF. Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment (Version 21) 2010.
    1. Dwyer CP, Hogan MJ, Stewart I. An evaluation of argument mapping as a method of enhancing critical thinking performance in e-learning environments. Metacognition Learn. 2012;7:219–44. 10.1007/s11409-012-9092-1.
    1. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45. 10.1177/1073191105283504.
    1. Sternberg S. Memory-scanning: new findings and current controversies. Q J Exp Psychol. 1975;
    1. Watson D, Clark LA. The Panas-X. Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule: expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1994.
    1. Simmons CA, Lehmann P. Tools for strengths-based assessment and evaluation. New York: Springer publishing company; 2012.
    1. Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5 10.1186/1477-7525-5-63.
    1. Stewart-Brown S, Janmohamed K. Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale. Edinburgh: NHS Health Scotland, University of Warwick and University of Edinburgh; 2008.
    1. Cacioppo JT, Petty RE. The need for cognition. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1982;42. 10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116.
    1. Tolentino E, Curry L, Leak G. Further validation of the short form of the need for cognition scale. Psychol Rep. 1990;66. 10.2466/pr0.1990.66.1.321.
    1. Stanovich KE, West RF. Reasoning Independently of Prior Belief and Individual Differences in Actively Open-Minded Thinking. J Educ Psychol. 1997;89:342–357. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342.
    1. Stanovich KE, West RF. Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Think Reason. 2007;13:225–47.
    1. Del Re AC, Flückiger C, Goldberg SB, Hoyt WT. Monitoring mindfulness practice quality: An important consideration in mindfulness practice. Psychother Res. 2013;23(1):55–56.
    1. Davis FD. User acceptance of information technology: system characteristics, user perceptions and behavioral impacts. Int J Man-Machine Stud. 1993;38(3):475–487. 10.1006/imms.1993.1022.
    1. Kirkpatrick T, Manoukian L, Dear BF, Johnston L, Titov N. A feasibility open trial of internet-delivered cognitive-behavioural therapy (iCBT) among consumers of a non-governmental mental health organisation with anxiety. PeerJ. 2013;1:e210. doi: 10.7717/peerj.210.
    1. Arbuckle J. Amos (Version 23.0). 2014.
    1. IBM Corp . IBM SPSS statistics for windows, Version 22.0. 2013.
    1. Iacobucci D, Saldanha N, Deng X. A meditation on mediation: evidence that structural equations models perform better than regressions. J Consum Psychol. 2007;17. 10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70020-7.
    1. Fish J, Brimson J, Lynch S. Mindfulness interventions delivered by technology without facilitator involvement: what research exists and what are the clinical outcomes? Mindfulness (N Y). 2016. 10.1007/s12671-016-0548-2.
    1. Crane RS, Kuyken W, Williams JMG, Hastings RP, Cooper L, Fennell MJV. Competence in teaching mindfulness-based courses: concepts, development and assessment. Mindfulness (N Y). 2012;3:76–84. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0073-2.
    1. Garrison KA, Pal P, Rojiani R, Dallery J, O’Malley SS, Brewer JA. A randomized controlled trial of smartphone-based mindfulness training for smoking cessation: a study protocol. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15:83. 10.1186/s12888-015-0468-z.
    1. Chittaro L, Vianello A. Evaluation of a mobile mindfulness app distributed through on-line stores: a 4-week study. Int J Hum Comput Stud. 2016;86:63–80. 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.09.004.
    1. Aikens KA, Astin J, Pelletier KR, Levanovich K, Baase CM, Park YY, et al. Mindfulness Goes to work: impact of an online workplace intervention. J Occup Environ Med. 2014;56:721–31.
    1. Cavanagh K, Strauss C, Cicconi F, Griffiths N, Wyper A, Jones F. A randomised controlled trial of a brief online mindfulness-based intervention. Behav Res Ther. 2013;51:573–8.
    1. Glück TM, Maercker A. A randomized controlled pilot study of a brief web-based mindfulness training. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:1–12.
    1. Levin ME, Pistorello J, Seeley JR, Hayes SC. Feasibility of a prototype web-based acceptance and commitment therapy prevention program for college students. J Am Coll Heal. 2014;62:20–30.
    1. Morledge TJ, Allexandre D, Fox E, Fu AZ, Higashi MK, Kruzikas DT, et al. Feasibility of an online mindfulness program for stress management - a randomized, controlled trial. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46:137–148. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9490-x.
    1. Wolever RQ, Bobinet KJ, McCabe K, Mackenzie ER, Fekete E, Kusnick CA, et al. Effective and viable mind-body stress reduction in the workplace: a randomized controlled trial. J Occup Health Psychol. 2012;17:246–258. doi: 10.1037/a0027278.
    1. Altschuler A, Rosenbaum E, Gordon P, Canales S, Avins AL. Audio recordings of mindfulness-based stress reduction training to improve cancer patients’ mood and quality of life---a pilot feasibility study. Support Care Cancer. 2012;20:1291–7. 10.1007/s00520-011-1216-7.
    1. Warnecke E, Quinn S, Ogden K, Towle N, Nelson MR. A randomised controlled trial of the effects of mindfulness practice on medical student stress levels. Med Educ. 2011;45:381–8. 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2010.03877.x.
    1. Ruscio AC, Muench C, Brede E, MacIntyre J, Waters AJ. Administration and assessment of brief mindfulness practice in the field: a feasibility study using ecological momentary assessment. Mindfulness (N Y). 2016:988–99. 10.1007/s12671-016-0538-4.
    1. Szabo A, Kocsis Á. Psychological effects of deep-breathing: the impact of expectancy-priming. Psychol Health Med. 2016:1–6. 10.1080/13548506.2016.1191656.
    1. Frewen P, Hargraves H, DePierro J, D’Andrea W, Flodrowski L. Meditation breath attention scores (MBAS): development and investigation of an internet-based assessment of focused attention during meditation practice. Psychol Assess. 2016;28:830–40. 10.1037/pas0000283.
    1. Frewen PA, Unholzer F, Logie-Hagan KR-J, Mac Kinley JD. Meditation Breath attention scores (MBAS): test–retest reliability and sensitivity to repeated practice. Mindfulness (N Y). 2012. 10.1007/s12671-012-0161-y.
    1. Watier N, Dubois M. The effects of a brief mindfulness exercise on executive attention and recognition memory. Mindfulness (N Y). 2016:745–53. 10.1007/s12671-016-0514-z.
    1. Boutron I, Moher D, Altman DG, Schulz KF, Ravaud P. Extending the CONSORT statement to trials reporting nonpharmacological treatments: extension and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2008;148. 10.7326/0003-4819-148-4-200802190-00008.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe