Sex, lies and fMRI--gender differences in neural basis of deception

Artur Marchewka, Katarzyna Jednorog, Marcel Falkiewicz, Wojciech Szeszkowski, Anna Grabowska, Iwona Szatkowska, Artur Marchewka, Katarzyna Jednorog, Marcel Falkiewicz, Wojciech Szeszkowski, Anna Grabowska, Iwona Szatkowska

Abstract

Deception has always been a part of human communication as it helps to promote self-presentation. Although both men and women are equally prone to try to manage their appearance, their strategies, motivation and eagerness may be different. Here, we asked if lying could be influenced by gender on both the behavioral and neural levels. To test whether the hypothesized gender differences in brain activity related to deceptive responses were caused by differential socialization in men and women, we administered the Gender Identity Inventory probing the participants' subjective social sex role. In an fMRI session, participants were instructed either to lie or to tell the truth while answering a questionnaire focusing on general and personal information. Only for personal information, we found differences in neural responses during instructed deception in men and women. The women vs. men direct contrast revealed no significant differences in areas of activation, but men showed higher BOLD signal compared to women in the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG). Moreover, this effect remained unchanged when self-reported psychological gender was controlled for. Thus, our study showed that gender differences in the neural processes engaged during falsifying personal information might be independent from socialization.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Behavioral results. Mean accuracy rates…
Figure 1. Behavioral results. Mean accuracy rates (A) and reaction times (B) of men and women for deceptive and truthful responses.
Error bars represent standard error of the mean. No between-gender effect reached significance.
Figure 2. Differential reaction times (RT) of…
Figure 2. Differential reaction times (RT) of men and women for general and personal information.
Error bars represent standard error of the mean. In men, differential RT for personal information were significantly longer than for general information, whereas in women no such differences were observed.
Figure 3. Brain regions showing increased activity…
Figure 3. Brain regions showing increased activity during lying compared to truth-telling.
The activations are superimposed on a Colin27 template image in the MNI space. The colored bar represents t-values; L – left side; R- right side.
Figure 4. Brain activation map contrasting personal…
Figure 4. Brain activation map contrasting personal “lie” versus general “lie”.
The activations are superimposed on a Colin27 template image in the MNI space. The colored bar represents t-values; L – left side; R- right side.
Figure 5. Differences between lying and truth-telling…
Figure 5. Differences between lying and truth-telling in men and women in the left middle frontal gyrus area.
The bar chart represents the mean contrast value of the cluster. The activation is superimposed on a Colin27 template image in the MNI space. The colored bar represents t-values; L – left side; R- right side.
Figure 6. Experimental design of the study.
Figure 6. Experimental design of the study.
The instruction was presented above the fixation point and, after 2 seconds, was followed by a question that appeared below the fixation point for 3 seconds. The inter-stimulus interval was varied from 8–12 s (for details see text).

References

    1. Ganis G, Keenan JP (2009) The cognitive neuroscience of deception. Soc Neurosc 4: 465–472.
    1. Tyler JM, Feldman RS (2004) Truth, lies, and self-presentation: How gender and anticipated future interaction relate to deceptive behavior. J Appl Soc Psychol 34: 2602–2615.
    1. DePaulo BM, Kashy DA, Kirkendol SE, Wyer MM, Epstein JA (1996) Lying in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol 70: 979–995.
    1. Ross M, Holmberg D (1990) Recounting the Past - Gender Differences in the Recall of Events in the History of a Close Relationship. Ontario Symposium P 6: 135–152.
    1. Tooke W, Camire L (1991) Patterns of Deception in Intersexual and Intrasexual Mating Strategies. EtholSociobiol 12: 345–364.
    1. DePaulo BM, Epstein JA, Wyer MM (1993) Sex differences in lying: How women and men deal with the dilemma of deceit. In: Lewis M, Saarni C, editors. Lying and deception in everyday life. New York: Guilford Press. 126–147.
    1. Feldman RS, Forrest JA, Happ BR (2002) Self-presentation and verbal deception: Do self-presenters lie more? Basic Appl Soc Psychol 24: 163–170.
    1. Dreber A, Johannesson M (2008) Gender differences in deception. Econ Lett 99: 197–199.
    1. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108: 624–652.
    1. Abe N (2009) The neurobiology of deception: evidence from neuroimaging and loss-of-function studies. Curr Opin Neurol 22: 594–600.
    1. Christ SE, Van Essen DC, Watson JM, Brubaker LE, McDermott KB (2009) The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cereb Cortex 19: 1557–1566.
    1. Spence SA, Farrow TF, Herford AE, Wilkinson ID, Zheng Y, et al. (2001) Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport 12: 2849–2853.
    1. Ganis G, Kosslyn SM, Stose S, Thompson WL, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2003) Neural correlates of different types of deception: an fMRI investigation. Cerebl Cortex 13: 830–836.
    1. Nunez JM, Casey BJ, Egner T, Hare T, Hirsch J (2005) Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control. Neuroimage 25: 267–277.
    1. Abe N, Suzuki M, Mori E, Itoh M, Fujii T (2007) Deceiving others: distinct neural responses of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala in simple fabrication and deception with social interactions. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 287–295.
    1. Sip KE, Roepstorff A, McGregor W, Frith CD (2008) Detecting deception: the scope and limits. Trends Cogn Sci 12: 48–53.
    1. Hines TM, Herman-Jaglińska A, Bednarek D, Grabowska A (1996) Sex differences in the processing of odd and even numbers. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 56: 263–266.
    1. Speck O, Ernst T, Braun J, Koch C, Miller E, et al. (2000) Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory. NeuroReport 11: 2581–2585.
    1. Bell EC, Willson MC, Wilman AH, Dave S, Silverstone PH (2006) Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 30: 529–538.
    1. Huselid RF, Cooper ML (1992) Gender roles as mediators of sex differences in adolescent alcohol use and abuse. J Health Soc Behav 33: 348–362.
    1. Thomas BS (1995) The Effectiveness of Selected Risk-Factors in Mediating Gender Differences in Drinking and Its Problems. J Adolesc Health 17: 91–98.
    1. Rosenblitt JC, Soler H, Johnson SE, Quadagno DM (2001) Sensation seeking and hormones in men and women: exploring the link. Hormon Behav 40: 396–402.
    1. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR (2003) Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: application of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 11: 210–217.
    1. Silverman IW (2003) Gender differences in resistance to temptation: Theories and evidence. Developmental Rev 23: 219–259.
    1. Li CS, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R (2006) Gender differences in the neural correlates of response inhibition during a stop signal task. Neuroimage 32: 1918–1929.
    1. Bauer K, DeVincentis D, Taber J (2008) Gender Differences in the Effects of Multi-tasking Performance. PSY 220: Research Design and Statistics. Available: . Accessed 2012 Aug 6.
    1. Greely HT, Illes J (2007) Neuroscience-based lie detection: the urgent need for regulation. Am J Law Med 33: 377–431.
    1. Abe N (2011) How the Brain Shapes Deception: An Integrated Review of the Literature. Neuroscientis 17: 560–574.
    1. Lee TMC, Lee TMY, Raine A, Chan CCH (2010) Lying about the Valence of Affective Pictures: An fMRI Study. PLoS ONE 5(8): e12291 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012291.
    1. Kaylor-Hughes CJ, Lankappa ST (2011) The functional anatomical distinction between truth telling and deception is preserved among people with schizophrenia. Crim Behav Ment Health 21: 8–20.
    1. Staunton C, Hammond S (2011) An Investigation of the Guilty Knowledge Test Polygraph Examination. J Crim Psychol 1: 1–14.
    1. Herman-Jeglińska A, Grabowska A, Dulko S (2002) Masculinity, femininity and transsexualism. Arch Sex Behav 31: 527–534.
    1. Bjorklund DF, Kipp K (1996) Parental investment theory and gender differences in the evolution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychol Bull 120: 163–188.
    1. Whitley BE, Nelson AB, Jones CJ (1999) Gender differences in cheating attitudes and classroom cheating behavior: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles 41: 657–680.
    1. Brett M, Jean-Luc Anton J-C, Valabregue R, Poline J-B (2002) Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. The 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain. NeuroImage 16: No 2.
    1. Buchsbaum BR, Greer S, Chang WL, Berman KF (2005) Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin card-sorting task and component processes. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 35–45.
    1. Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, et al. (2006) A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50: 799–812.
    1. Lee TM, Liu HL, Tan LH, Chan CC, Mahankali S, et al. (2002) Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 15: 157–164.
    1. Karim AA, Schneider M, Lotze M, Veit R, Sauseng P, et al. (2010) The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive behavior. Cereb Cortex 20: 205–213.
    1. Paulus MP, Frank LR (2003) Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is critical for preference judgments. NeuroReport 14: 1311–1315.
    1. Menon V, Uddin LQ (2010) Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 214: 655–667.
    1. Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Paradiso S, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, et al. (1999) The cerebellum plays a role in conscious episodic memory retrieval. Hum Brain Mapp 8: 226–234.
    1. Marvel CL, Desmond JE (2010) Functional topography of the cerebellum in verbal working memory. Neuropsychol Rev 20: 271–279.
    1. Graybiel AM (2005) The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 638–644.
    1. Kenner NM, Mumford JA, Hommer RE, Skup M, Leibenluft E, et al. (2010) Inhibitory motor control in response stopping and response switching. J Neurosci 30: 8512–8518.
    1. Abe N, Suzuki M, Tsukiura T, Mori E, Yamaguchi K, et al. (2006) Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. Cereb Cortex 16: 192–199.
    1. Abe N, Fujii T, Hirayama K, Takeda A, Hosokai Y, et al. (2009) Do parkinsonian patients have trouble telling lies? The neurobiological basis of deceptive behaviour. Brain 132: 1386–1395.
    1. Jonides J, Smith EE, Marshuetz C, Koeppe RA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, et al. (1998) Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 8410–8413.
    1. D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Jonides J, Smith EE (1999) The neural substrate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI., Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 96: 7514–7519.
    1. Thompson-Schill SL, Jonides J, Marshuetz C, Smith EE, D’Esposito M, et al. (2002) Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2: 109–120.
    1. Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, Eddy WF, Thulborn KR (1996) Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274: 114–116.
    1. Sip KE, Lynge M, Wallentin M, McGregor WB, Frith CD, et al. (2010) The production and detection of deception in an interactive game. Neuropsychologia 48: 3619–3626.
    1. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.
    1. Kuczyńska A (1992) Inwentarz do oceny płci psychologicznej. Podręcznik. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.
    1. Bem SL (1981) Bem sex role inventory professional manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
    1. Szatkowska I, Szymańska O, Marchewka A, Soluch P, Rymarczyk K (2011) Dissociable contributions of the left and right posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex in motivational control of goal-directed behavior. Neurobiol Learn Mem 96: 385–391.
    1. Szatkowska I, Bogorodzki P, Wolak T, Marchewka A, Szeszkowski W (2008) The effect of motivation on working memory: An fMRI and SEM study. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90: 475–480.
    1. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38: 95–113.
    1. Eickhoff SB, Heim S, Zilles K, Amunts K (2006) Testing anatomically specified hypotheses in functional imaging using cytoarchitectonic maps. NeuroImage 32: 570–582.
    1. Eickhoff SB, Paus T, Caspers S, Grosbras MH, Evans AC, et al. (2007) Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. Neuroimage 36: 511–521.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe