Replay of Learned Neural Firing Sequences during Rest in Human Motor Cortex

Jean-Baptiste Eichenlaub, Beata Jarosiewicz, Jad Saab, Brian Franco, Jessica Kelemen, Eric Halgren, Leigh R Hochberg, Sydney S Cash, Jean-Baptiste Eichenlaub, Beata Jarosiewicz, Jad Saab, Brian Franco, Jessica Kelemen, Eric Halgren, Leigh R Hochberg, Sydney S Cash

Abstract

The offline "replay" of neural firing patterns underlying waking experience, previously observed in non-human animals, is thought to be a mechanism for memory consolidation. Here, we test for replay in the human brain by recording spiking activity from the motor cortex of two participants who had intracortical microelectrode arrays placed chronically as part of a brain-computer interface pilot clinical trial. Participants took a nap before and after playing a neurally controlled sequence-copying game that consists of many repetitions of one "repeated" sequence sparsely interleaved with varying "control" sequences. Both participants performed repeated sequences more accurately than control sequences, consistent with learning. We compare the firing rate patterns that caused the cursor movements when performing each sequence to firing rate patterns throughout both rest periods. Correlations with repeated sequences increase more from pre- to post-task rest than do correlations with control sequences, providing direct evidence of learning-related replay in the human brain.

Keywords: brain-computer interface; consolidation; human; learning; memory; microelectrode array; motor cortex; reactivation; replay; sleep.

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Interests B.J. and B.F. are currently employees of NeuroPace, Inc., and hold stock options in the company. The MGH Translational Research Center has a clinical research support agreement with Neuralink, Paradromics, and Synchron, for which L.R.H. and S.S.C. provide consultative input.

Copyright © 2020 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Figures

Figure 1.. Research Session Setup
Figure 1.. Research Session Setup
(A) Task and timeline. In each session, a decoder was initialized and calibrated using an open-loop and then closed-loop center-out task (see Method Details; calibration inset modified from Jarosiewicz et al., 2015). The participant was then invited to relax with his eyes closed, and nap if desired, during a pre-task rest period (Rest1). Following Rest1, the participant performed the 4-target sequence game, consisting of 66 presentations of the repeated sequence (in this example, red-teal-yellow-blue), interspersed with 22 control sequences. Following the game, the participant was invited to relax with his eyes closed again (Rest2). (B) Participant T9 at his home during a research session. (C) Performance in the game, divided into control and repeated sequences. Each line represents one session from one participant (orange = T9 sessions; cornflower blue = T10 sessions). The success rate (% of sequences correctly completed) was significantly higher for the repeated than the control sequences (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, n = 10, p = 0.0039), suggesting preferential learning of the repeated sequences.
Figure 2.. Replay of Firing Rate Patterns…
Figure 2.. Replay of Firing Rate Patterns in Motor Cortex
(A) Examples of distributions of RIs. One value in each distribution is the RI for one trial (the % change in mean peak CC values from Rest1 to Rest2 when using that trial as the template). The red distributions were obtained using the repeated sequence trials as templates, and the blue distributions were obtained using the control trials. Neural signal nonstationarities can cause spurious drifts in CC values; thus, replay was measured as the difference between the repeated and the control trials’ RIs. In both example sessions (one from each participant), the repeated sequence trials (mean ± SEM in red) showed significantly higher (i.e., more positive) RIs than the control sequence trials (in blue; two-sample one-tailed t test). (B) Difference in mean RI (repeated – control) for each session at all time dilation factors (using a CC threshold at the 95th centile). Sessions with T9 are shown in orange and those with T10 in cornflower blue. The mean across sessions is shown with a thick black line. Asterisks denote time dilation factors at which the repeated-trial mean RIs were significantly higher than the control-trial mean RIs across sessions (paired one-tailed t test, n = 10; *p < 0.01). (C and D) The t scores (C) and corresponding p values (D) resulting from testing, for each time dilation factor and centile threshold, whether the repeated-trial mean RIs were higher than the control-trial mean RIs across sessions (paired one-tailed t test, n = 10). Note that the middle columns of (C) and (D) correspond to the data shown in (B).
Figure 3.. Replay Results across Sessions, with…
Figure 3.. Replay Results across Sessions, with Rest Periods Subdivided into Putative NREM1 and Putative Waking States
(A–C) Analagous to Figures 2B–2D, showing results for putative NREM1. (D–F) Analagous to Figures 2B–2D, showing results for putative waking. *p

References

    1. Abel T, Havekes R, Saletin JM, and Walker MP (2013). Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Curr. Biol 23, R774–R788.
    1. Aflalo T, Kellis S, Klaes C, Lee B, Shi Y, Pejsa K, Shanfield K, Hayes-Jackson S, Aisen M, Heck C, et al. (2015). Neurophysiology. Decoding motor imagery from the posterior parietal cortex of a tetraplegic human. Science 348, 906–910.
    1. Ajiboye AB, Willett FR, Young DR, Memberg WD, Murphy BA, Miller JP, Walter BL, Sweet JA, Hoyen HA, Keith MW, et al. (2017). Restoration of reaching and grasping movements through brain-controlled muscle stimulation in a person with tetraplegia: a proof-of-concept demonstration. Lancet 389, 1821–1830.
    1. Atherton LA, Dupret D, and Mellor JR (2015). Memory trace replay: the shaping of memory consolidation by neuromodulation. Trends Neurosci. 38, 560–570.
    1. Bouton CE, Shaikhouni A, Annetta NV, Bockbrader MA, Friedenberg DA, Nielson DM, Sharma G, Sederberg PB, Glenn BC, Mysiw WJ, et al. (2016). Restoring cortical control of functional movement in a human with quadriplegia. Nature 533, 247–250.
    1. Buzsáki G (2015). Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus 25, 1073, 188.
    1. Carr MF, Jadhav SP, and Frank LM (2011). Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat. Neurosci 14, 147–153.
    1. Cash SS, and Hochberg LR (2015). The emergence of single neurons in clinical neurology. Neuron 86, 79–91.
    1. Chestek CA, Gilja V, Nuyujukian P, Foster JD, Fan JM, Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Rivera-Alvidrez Z, Cunningham JP, Ryu SI, and Shenoy KV (2011). Long-term stability of neural prosthetic control signals from silicon cortical arrays in rhesus macaque motor cortex. J. Neural Eng 8, 045005.
    1. Christie BP, Tat DM, Irwin ZT, Gilja V, Nuyujukian P, Foster JD, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV, Thompson DE, and Chestek CA (2015). Comparison of spike sorting and thresholding of voltage waveforms for intracortical brain-machine interface performance. J. Neural Eng 12, 016009.
    1. Collinger JL, Wodlinger B, Downey JE, Wang W, Tyler-Kabara EC, Weber DJ, McMorland AJC, Velliste M, Boninger ML, and Schwartz AB (2013). High-performance neuroprosthetic control by an individual with tetraplegia. Lancet 381, 557–564.
    1. Csicsvari J, O’Neill J, Allen K, and Senior T (2007). Place-selective firing contributes to the reverse-order reactivation of CA1 pyramidal cells during sharp waves in open-field exploration. Eur. J. Neurosci 26, 704–716.
    1. Davidson TJ, Kloosterman F, and Wilson MA (2009). Hippocampal replay of extended experience. Neuron 63, 497–507.
    1. Deuker L, Olligs J, Fell J, Kranz TA, Mormann F, Montag C, Reuter M, Elger CE, and Axmacher N (2013). Memory consolidation by replay of stimulus-specific neural activity. J. Neurosci 33, 19373–19383.
    1. Diba K, and Buzsáki G (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat. Neurosci 10, 1241–1242.
    1. Diekelmann S, Büchel C, Born J, and Rasch B (2011). Labile or stable: opposing consequences for memory when reactivated during waking and sleep. Nat. Neurosci 14, 381–386.
    1. Euston DR, Tatsuno M, and McNaughton BL (2007). Fast-forward playback of recent memory sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Science 318, 1147–1150.
    1. Felzenszwalb PF, Girshick RB, McAllester D, and Ramanan D (2010). Object detection with discriminatively trained part-based models. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell 32, 1627–1645.
    1. Foster DJ, and Wilson MA (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature 440, 680–683.
    1. Fraser GW, Chase SM, Whitford A, and Schwartz AB (2009). Control of a brain-computer interface without spike sorting. J. Neural Eng 6, 055004.
    1. Gilja V, Pandarinath C, Blabe CH, Nuyujukian P, Simeral JD, Sarma AA, Sorice BL, Perge JA, Jarosiewicz B, Hochberg LR, et al. (2015). Clinical translation of a high-performance neural prosthesis. Nat. Med 21, 1142–1145.
    1. Gonzalez CE, Mak-McCully RA, Rosen BQ, Cash SS, Chauvel PY, Bastuji H, Rey M, and Halgren E (2018). Theta Bursts Precede, and Spindles Follow, Cortical and Thalamic Downstates in Human NREM Sleep. J. Neurosci 38, 9989–10001.
    1. Gulati T, Ramanathan DS, Wong CC, and Ganguly K (2014). Reactivation of emergent task-related ensembles during slow-wave sleep after neuroprosthetic learning. Nat. Neurosci 17, 1107–1113.
    1. Hayashi M, Motoyoshi N, and Hori T (2005). Recuperative Power of a Short Daytime Nap With or Without Stage 2 Sleep. Sleep 28, 829–836.
    1. Hochberg LR, Serruya MD, Friehs GM, Mukand JA, Saleh M, Caplan AH, Branner A, Chen D, Penn RD, and Donoghue JP (2006). Neuronal ensemble control of prosthetic devices by a human with tetraplegia. Nature 442, 164–171.
    1. Hochberg LR, Bacher D, Jarosiewicz B, Masse NY, Simeral JD, Vogel J, Haddadin S, Liu J, Cash SS, van der Smagt P, and Donoghue JP (2012). Reach and grasp by people with tetraplegia using a neurally controlled robotic arm. Nature 485, 372–375.
    1. Iber C, Ancoli-lsrael S, Chesson AL, and Quan SF (2007). The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology, and technical specifications (American Academy of Sleep Medicine).
    1. Jarosiewicz B, Masse NY, Bacher D, Cash SS, Eskandar E, Friehs G, Donoghue JP, and Hochberg LR (2013). Advantages of closed-loop calibration in intracortical brain-computer interfaces for people with tetraplegia. J. Neural Eng 10, 046012.
    1. Jarosiewicz B, Sarma AA, Bacher D, Masse NY, Simeral JD, Sorice B, Oakley EM, Blabe C, Pandarinath C, Gilja V, et al. (2015). Virtual typing by people with tetraplegia using a self-calibrating intracortical brain-computer interface. Sci. Transl. Med 7, 313ra179.
    1. Jarosiewicz B, Sarma AA, Saab J, Franco B, Cash SS, Eskandar EN, and Hochberg LR (2016). Retrospectively supervised click decoder calibration for self-calibrating point-and-click brain-computer interfaces. J. Physiol. Paris 110, 382–391.
    1. Ji D, and Wilson MA (2007). Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat. Neurosci 10, 100–107.
    1. Jiang X, Shamie I, K Doyle W, Friedman D, Dugan P, Devinsky O, Eskandar E, Cash SS, Thesen T, and Halgren E (2017). Replay of large-scale spatio-temporal patterns from waking during subsequent NREM sleep in human cortex. Sci. Rep 7, 17380.
    1. Jiang X, Gonzalez-Martinez J, and Halgren E (2019a). Coordination of human hippocampal sharpwave ripples during NREM sleep with cortical theta bursts, spindles, downstates, and upstates. J. Neurosci 39, 8744–8761.
    1. Jiang X, Gonzalez-Martinez J, and Halgren E (2019b). Posterior hippocampal spindle-ripples co-occur with neocortical theta-bursts and down-upstates, and phase-lock with parietal spindles during NREM sleep in humans. J. Neurosci. 39, 8949–8968.
    1. Johnson LA, Euston DR, Tatsuno M, and McNaughton BL (2010). Stored-trace reactivation in rat prefrontal cortex is correlated with down-to-up state fluctuation density. J. Neurosci 30, 2650–2661.
    1. Karlsson MP, and Frank LM (2009). Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci 12, 913–918.
    1. Kim S-P, Simeral JD, Hochberg LR, Donoghue JP, and Black MJ (2008). Neural control of computer cursor velocity by decoding motor cortical spiking activity in humans with tetraplegia. J. Neural Eng 5, 455–476.
    1. Lansink CS, Goltstein PM, Lankelma JV, McNaughton BL, and Pennartz CMA (2009). Hippocampus leads ventral striatum in replay of place-reward information. PLoS Biol. 7, e1000173.
    1. Lee AK, and Wilson MA (2002). Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, 1183–1194.
    1. Louie K, and Wilson MA (2001). Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 29, 145–156.
    1. Ludwig KA, Miriani RM, Langhals NB, Joseph MD, Anderson DJ, and Kipke DR (2009). Using a common average reference to improve cortical neuron recordings from microelectrode arrays. J. Neurophysiol 101, 1679–1689.
    1. Magnin M, Rey M, Bastuji H, Guillemant P, Mauguiere F, and Garcia-Larrea L (2010). Thalamic deactivation at sleep onset precedes that of the cerebral cortex in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 3829–3833.
    1. Malik WQ, Truccolo W, Brown EN, and Hochberg LR (2011). Efficient decoding with steady-state Kalman filter in neural interface systems. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng 19, 25–34.
    1. Malik WQ, Hochberg LR, Donoghue JP, and Brown EN (2015). Modulation depth estimation and variable selection in state-space models for neural interfaces. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng 62, 570–581.
    1. Maquet P, Laureys S, Peigneux P, Fuchs S, Petiau C, Phillips C, Aerts J, Del Fiore G, Degueldre C, Meulemans T, et al. (2000). Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nat. Neurosci 3, 831–836.
    1. Masse NY, Jarosiewicz B, Simeral JD, Bacher D, Stavisky SD, Cash SS, Oakley EM, Berhanu E, Eskandar E, Friehs G, et al. (2014). Non-causal spike filtering improves decoding of movement intention for intracortical BCIs. J. Neurosci. Methods 236, 58–67.
    1. Nuyujukian P, Albites Sanabria J, Saab J, Pandarinath C, Jarosiewicz B, Blabe CH, Franco B, Mernoff ST, Eskandar EN, Simeral JD, et al. (2018). Cortical control of a tablet computer by people with paralysis. PLoS ONE 13, e0204566.
    1. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, and Schoffelen J-M (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci 2011, 156869.
    1. Pavlides C, and Winson J (1989). Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. J. Neurosci 9, 2907–2918.
    1. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, Collette F, Perrin F, Reggers J, Phillips C, Degueldre C, Del Fiore G, Aerts J, et al. (2004). Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron 44, 535–545.
    1. Perge JA, Homer ML, Malik WQ, Cash S, Eskandar E, Friehs G, Donoghue JP, and Hochberg LR (2013). Intra-day signal instabilities affect decoding performance in an intracortical neural interface system. J. Neural Eng 10, 036004.
    1. Piantoni G, Van DerWerf YD, Jensen O, and Van Someren EJW (2015). Memory traces of long-range coordinated oscillations in the sleeping human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp 36, 67–84.
    1. Ramanathan DS, Gulati T, and Ganguly K (2015). Sleep-Dependent Reactivation of Ensembles in Motor Cortex Promotes Skill Consolidation. PLoS Biol 13, e1002263.
    1. Rasch B, Biichel C, Gais S, and Born J (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science 315, 1426–1429.
    1. Ribeiro S, Gervasoni D, Soares ES, Zhou Y, Lin S-C, Pantoja J, Lavine M, and Nicolelis MAL (2004). Long-lasting novelty-induced neuronal reverberation during slow-wave sleep in multiple forebrain areas. PLoS Biol. 2, E24.
    1. Rothe R, Guillaumin M, and Van Gool L (2015). Non-maximum suppression for object detection by passing messages between windows. Computer Vision-ACCV 2014 (Springer International Publishing; ), pp. 290–306.
    1. Schapiro AC, McDevitt EA, Rogers TT, Mednick SC, and Norman KA (2018). Human hippocampal replay during rest prioritizes weakly learned information and predicts memory performance. Nat. Commun 9, 3920.
    1. Skaggs WE, and McNaughton BL (1996). Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science 271, 1870–1873.
    1. Squire LR, Genzel L, Wixted JT, and Morris RG (2015). Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol 7, a021766.
    1. Staresina BP, Alink A, Kriegeskorte N, and Henson RN (2013). Awake reactivation predicts memory in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 21159–21164.
    1. Tambini A, and Davachi L (2013). Persistence of hippocampal multivoxel patterns into postencoding rest is related to memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110, 19591–19596.
    1. Tatsuno M, Lipa P, and McNaughton BL (2006). Methodological considerations on the use of template matching to study long-lasting memory trace replay. J. Neurosci 26, 10727–10742.
    1. Truccolo W, Friehs GM, Donoghue JP, and Hochberg LR (2008). Primary motor cortex tuning to intended movement kinematics in humans with tetraplegia. J. Neurosci 28, 1163–1178.
    1. Wilber AA, Skelin I, Wu W, and McNaughton BL (2017). Laminar Organization of Encoding and Memory Reactivation in the Parietal Cortex. Neuron 95, 1406–1419.e5.
    1. Wilson MA, and McNaughton BL (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, 676–679.
    1. Wu W, Gao Y, Bienenstock E, Donoghue JP, and Black MJ (2006). Bayesian population decoding of motor cortical activity using a Kalman filter. Neural Comput. 18, 80–118.
    1. Yousry TA, Schmid UD, Alkadhi H, Schmidt D, Peraud A, Buettner A, and Winkler P (1997). Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus. Anew landmark. Brain 120, 141–157.
    1. Zhang H, Deuker L, and Axmacher N (2017). Replay in humans–first evidence and open questions In Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation, Axmacher N and Rasch B, eds. (Springer International Publishing), pp. 251–263.
    1. Zhang H, Fell J, and Axmacher N (2018). Electrophysiological mechanisms of human memory consolidation. Nat. Commun 9, 4103.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe