A group study on the effects of a short multi-domain cognitive training in healthy elderly Italian people

Chiara F Tagliabue, Sabrina Guzzetti, Giulia Gualco, Giovanna Boccolieri, Alfonsa Boccolieri, Stuart Smith, Roberta Daini, Chiara F Tagliabue, Sabrina Guzzetti, Giulia Gualco, Giovanna Boccolieri, Alfonsa Boccolieri, Stuart Smith, Roberta Daini

Abstract

Background: Alongside physiological cognitive ageing, nowadays there is an alarming increase in the incidence of dementia that requires communities to invest in its prevention. The engagement in cognitively stimulating activities and strong social networks has been identified among those protective factors promoting successful cognitive ageing. One aspect regarding cognitive stimulation concerns the relevance of the frequency of an external intervention. For this reason, the aim of this study was to evaluate the efficacy of a 3-month cognitive training program, once per week, in a group of healthy elderly aged over 60 years old. Their results were compared with those of a passive control group.

Methods: The training consisted of a weekly session of multi-domain and ecological cognitive exercises performed in small homogenous (i.e. same cognitive level) groups. The scores obtained in a neuropsychological assessment by the experimental and control groups were compared at pre- and post-training. In addition, by means of a questionnaire, we also evaluated the indirect effect of the program on participants' mood, socialization and perceived impact on everyday activities.

Results: Overall, the experimental group showed a general improvement in cognitive functioning following the training program, even with the frequency of once per week. Greater improvements were observed mainly on executive functions and short-term memory, but general cognitive functioning and non-verbal reasoning also showed a tendency to an improvement. It is noteworthy that a majority of the participants reported to have subjectively experienced an improvement in their everyday life and a positive influence on both mood and socialization.

Conclusions: These results show that even a low-intensity training program is able to promote some of the protective factors that support successful cognitive ageing. Moreover, this multi-domain approach proved to be an excellent training method to transfer gains not only to other cognitive domains, but also to everyday living.

Trial registration: NCT03771131 ; the study was retrospectively registered on December 7th 2018.

Keywords: Active ageing; Cognitive stimulation; Training intensity.

Conflict of interest statement

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The project was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Milano-Bicocca. All the participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Neuropsychological assessment. Mean raw scores (and standard errors) at each neuropsychological test [a - n] for both Experimental (blue line) and Control group (red line) at pre- (1) and post-testing (2)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Numerosity of maximum equivalent scores. Mean sum of neuropsychological tests receiving an equivalent score of 4 for both Experimental (blue line) and Control Group (red line) at pre- (1) and post-training (2)
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Training effects (Questionnaire). Impact of the training on daily living, mood, and socialization as assessed by a questionnaire after the training

References

    1. Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):87–97.
    1. Cattell RB. Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. New York: Houghton Mifflin; 1971.
    1. Park DC, Bischof GN. The aging mind: neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15(1):109.
    1. Dartigues JF. Alzheimer's disease: a global challenge for the 21st century. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(12):1082–1083.
    1. Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, et al. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet. 2005;366(9503):2112–2117.
    1. Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci L, Lepore V, Bracco L, Maggi S, et al. Incidence of dementia, Alzheimer's disease, and vascular dementia in Italy. The ILSA study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(1):41–48.
    1. Luengo-Fernandez R, Leal J, Gray AM. UK research expenditure on dementia, heart disease, stroke and cancer: are levels of spending related to disease burden? Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(1):149–154.
    1. Middleton LE, Yaffe K. Promising strategies for the prevention of dementia. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(10):1210.
    1. Daffner KR. Promoting successful cognitive aging: a comprehensive review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19(4):1101–1122.
    1. Potter GG, Steffens DC. Contribution of depression to cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults. Neurologist. 2007;13(3):105–117.
    1. Valenzuela MJ, Breakspear M, Sachdev P. Complex mental activity and the aging brain: molecular, cellular and cortical network mechanisms. Brain Res Rev. 2007;56(1):198–213.
    1. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(10):2015–2028.
    1. Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, Brennan S. The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;15:28–43.
    1. Lampit A, Hallock H, Suo C, Naismith SL, Valenzuela M. Cognitive training-induced short-term functional and long-term structural plastic change is related to gains in global cognition in healthy older adults: a pilot study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;9:7–14.
    1. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Everyday cognition: age and intellectual ability correlates. Psychol Aging. 1999;14(4):627–644.
    1. Stuss DT, Robertson IH, Craik FIM, Levine B, Alexander MP, Black S, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: a randomized trial to evaluate a new protocol. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(1):120–131.
    1. Lusting C, Priti S, Seodler R, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19(4):504–522.
    1. Rebok GW, Carlson MC, Langbaurn JBS. Training and maintaining memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel approaches. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62:53–61.
    1. Noice H, Noice T, Staines G. A short-term intervention to enhance cognitive and affective functioning in older adults. J Aging Health. 2004;16(4):562–585.
    1. Tranter LJ, Koutstaal W. Age and flexible thinking: an experimental demonstration of the beneficial effects of increased cognitively stimulating activity on fluid intelligence in healthy older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2008;15(2):184–207.
    1. Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and visual-search—expanding the useful field of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5(12):2210–2219.
    1. Karbach J, Kray J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev Sci. 2009;12(6):978–990.
    1. Cassavaugh ND, Kramer AF. Transfer of computer-based cognitive training to simulated driving in older adults. Appl Ergon. 2009;40(5):943–952.
    1. Kramer AF, Larish JF, Strayer DL. Training for attentional control in dual-task settings—a comparison of young and old adults. J Exp Psychol Appl. 1995;1(1):50–76.
    1. Bellander M, Eschen A, Lövdén M, Martin M, Bäckman L, Brehmer Y. No evidence for improved associative Memory performance following process-based associative Memory training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2016;8:326.
    1. Cheng Y, Wu W, Feng W, Wang J, Chen Y, Shen Y, Li Q, et al. The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain cognitive training in non-demented older people: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Med. 2012;10(1):10–30.
    1. Binder JC, Martin M, Zöllig J, Röcke C, Mérillat S, Eschen A, et al. Multi-domain training enhances attentional control. Psychol Aging. 2016;31(4):390–408.
    1. Binder JC, Bezzola L, Haueter AIS, Klein C, Kühnis J, Baetschmann H, Jäncke L. Expertise-related functional brain network efficiency in healthy older adults. BMC Neurosci. 2017;18:2.
    1. Lövdén M, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U, Schaefer S, Schmiedek F. A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(4):659–676.
    1. Ball K, Edwards JD, Ross LA. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62(Special Issue 1):19–31.
    1. Kim GH, Jeon S, Im K, Kwon H, Lee BH, Kim GY, Jeong H, Han NE, Seo SW, Cho H, Noh Y, Park SE, Kim H, Hwang JW, Yoon CW, Kim HJ, Ye BS, Chin JH, Kim JH, Suh MK, Lee JM, Kim ST, Choi MT, Kim MS, Heilman KM, Jeong JH, Na DL. Structural brain changes after traditional and robot-assisted multi-domain cognitive training in community-dwelling healthy elderly. PLoS One. 2015;10(4):e0123251.
    1. Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M. Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 2014;11(11):e1001756.
    1. Reijnders JSAM, Geusgens CAV, Ponds RWHM, van Boxtel MPJ. “Keep your brain fit!” effectiveness of a psychoeducational intervention on cognitive functioning in healthy adults: a randomised controlled trial. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2015;28:1–17.
    1. Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Evans DA. Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology. 2004;63(12):2322–2326.
    1. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Chertkow H. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699.
    1. Conti S, Bonazzi S, Laiacona M, Masina M, Coralli MV. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA)-Italian version: regression based norms and equivalent scores. Neurol Sci. 2015;36(2):209–214.
    1. Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci. 2013;34(5):749–754.
    1. Novelli G, Papagno C, Capitani E, Laiacona M. Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su sogetti normali. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47(4):477–506.
    1. Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e Taratura Italiana di Test Neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;S8 Suppl 6:8–20.
    1. Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. 2002;22(6):443–447.
    1. Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Una versione abbreviata del test di Stroop: dati normativi nella popolazione italiana. Nuova Rivista di Neurologia. 2002;12(4):111–115.
    1. Giovagnoli AR, Del Pesce M, Mascheroni S, Simoncelli M, Laiacona M, Capitani E. Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1996;17(4):305–309.
    1. Basso A, Capitani E, Laiacona M. Raven's coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls. Funct Neurol. 1986;2(2):189–194.
    1. Austin-Wells V, Zimmerman T, McDougall GJ. An optimal delivery format for presentations targeting older adults. Educ Gerontol. 2003;29(6):493–501.
    1. Buckner RL. Memory and Executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44(1):195–208.
    1. Tomaszewski Farias S, Cahn-Weiner DA, Harvey DJ, Reed BR, Mungas D, Kramer JH, Chui H. Longitudinal changes in memory and executive functioning are associated with longitudinal change in instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;23(3):446–461.
    1. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–186.
    1. Willis SL, Marsiske M. Manual for the everyday problems test. University Park: Pennsylvania State University; 1993.
    1. Bandura A. Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Dev Psychol. 1989;25(5):729–735.
    1. West RL, Thorn RM, Bagwell DK. Memory performance and beliefs as a function of goal setting and aging. Psychol Aging. 2003;18(1):111–125.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnieren