Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients

Alexander R Green, Dana R Carney, Daniel J Pallin, Long H Ngo, Kristal L Raymond, Lisa I Iezzoni, Mahzarin R Banaji, Alexander R Green, Dana R Carney, Daniel J Pallin, Long H Ngo, Kristal L Raymond, Lisa I Iezzoni, Mahzarin R Banaji

Abstract

Context: Studies documenting racial/ethnic disparities in health care frequently implicate physicians' unconscious biases. No study to date has measured physicians' unconscious racial bias to test whether this predicts physicians' clinical decisions.

Objective: To test whether physicians show implicit race bias and whether the magnitude of such bias predicts thrombolysis recommendations for black and white patients with acute coronary syndromes.

Design, setting, and participants: An internet-based tool comprising a clinical vignette of a patient presenting to the emergency department with an acute coronary syndrome, followed by a questionnaire and three Implicit Association Tests (IATs). Study invitations were e-mailed to all internal medicine and emergency medicine residents at four academic medical centers in Atlanta and Boston; 287 completed the study, met inclusion criteria, and were randomized to either a black or white vignette patient.

Main outcome measures: IAT scores (normal continuous variable) measuring physicians' implicit race preference and perceptions of cooperativeness. Physicians' attribution of symptoms to coronary artery disease for vignette patients with randomly assigned race, and their decisions about thrombolysis. Assessment of physicians' explicit racial biases by questionnaire.

Results: Physicians reported no explicit preference for white versus black patients or differences in perceived cooperativeness. In contrast, IATs revealed implicit preference favoring white Americans (mean IAT score = 0.36, P < .001, one-sample t test) and implicit stereotypes of black Americans as less cooperative with medical procedures (mean IAT score 0.22, P < .001), and less cooperative generally (mean IAT score 0.30, P < .001). As physicians' prowhite implicit bias increased, so did their likelihood of treating white patients and not treating black patients with thrombolysis (P = .009).

Conclusions: This study represents the first evidence of unconscious (implicit) race bias among physicians, its dissociation from conscious (explicit) bias, and its predictive validity. Results suggest that physicians' unconscious biases may contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in use of medical procedures such as thrombolysis for myocardial infarction.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Implicit Association Test (IAT) sample screens and stimuli. This figure displays sample screens and stimuli from the race preference (black-white/good-bad) IAT. Sample screens a, b, c, and d represent examples of pairing tasks that participants rapidly complete. Pictures of black or white individuals and words representing good or bad evaluative attributes are flashed in the center of the screen, and subjects quickly classify these as to whether they belong with category pairs shown in the upper left or the upper right of their screens using the e or i key on their computer keyboard. Numerous pictures and words are flashed onscreen for each of the two possible pairings, with responses usually taking less than a second and the order counterbalanced across participants. The speed to associate black+bad and white+good (screens a and b) relative to the opposite pairing of black+good and white+bad (screens c and d) constitutes the IAT score, interpreted to be a measure of implicit race preference
Figure 2
Figure 2
Magnitude of physicians’ explicit (self-reported) and implicit (Implicit Association Test) race bias on a standardized scale—Cohen’s effect size d
Figure 3
Figure 3
Relationship between physician race preference Implicit Association Test (IAT) score and thrombolysis decisions by patient race. *P < .05, **P = .05–0.11, B values are standardized regression coefficients that describe the magnitude of each relationship that the regression lines represent. IAT bias is a continuous variable represented on the polar ends of the x-axis as low antiblack IAT and high antiblack IAT. Treatment recommendation of thrombolysis is represented on the y-axis and is a dichotomous variable for which 0 means “would not give thrombolysis” and 1 means “would give thrombolysis.” Subpanels AD represent race preference, general cooperativeness, medical cooperativeness, and the composite IAT measures, respectively
Figure 4
Figure 4
Relation between physicians’ awareness of the study’s purpose and Implicit Association Test (IAT) bias on recommendation for thrombolysis (black patients only). B values are standardized regression coefficients that describe the magnitude of each relationship that the regression lines represent (P = .001). IAT bias is a continuous variable represented on the polar ends of the x-axis as low antiblack IAT and high antiblack IAT. Treatment recommendation of thrombolysis is represented on the y-axis and is a dichotomous variable for which 1 means “no recommendation” was given and 2 means a “recommendation” was given

References

    1. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson, AR, eds. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. Washington, DC: Institute of Medicine; 2003.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11529699', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11529699/'}]}
    2. Kressin NR, Petersen LA. Racial differences in the use of invasive cardiovascular procedures: review of the literature and prescription for future research. Ann Intern Med. 2001;135(5):352–66.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11789635', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11789635/'}]}
    2. Petersen LA, Wright SM, Peterson ED, Daley J. Impact of race on cardiac care and outcomes in veterans with acute myocardial infarction. Med Care. 2002;40(1 suppl):I86–96.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1007/BF02598987', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02598987'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '9016420', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9016420/'}]}
    2. Allison JJ, Kiefe CI, Centor RM, Box JB, Farmer RM. Racial differences in the medical treatment of elderly Medicare patients with acute myocardial infarction. J Gen Intern Med. 1996;11(12):736–43.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1056/NEJM200004133421505', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1056/nejm200004133421505'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10760310', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10760310/'}]}
    2. Canto JG, Allison JJ, Kiefe CI, et al. Relation of race and sex to the use of reperfusion therapy in medicare beneficiaries with acute myocardial infarction. N Engl J Med. 2000;342(15):1094–100.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0002-9149(96)00857-0', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9149(96)00857-0'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '9070548', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9070548/'}]}
    2. Weitzman S, Cooper L, Chambless L, Rosamond W, Clegg L. Gender, racial, and geographic differences in the performance of cardiac diagnostic and therapeutic procedures for hospitalized acute myocardial infarction in four states. Am J Cardiol. 1997;79(6):722–6.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0002-9149(98)00547-5', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9149(98)00547-5'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '9817474', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9817474/'}]}
    2. Taylor JHA, Canto JG, Sanderson B, Rogers WJ, Hilbe J. Management and outcomes for black patients with acute myocardial infarction in the reperfusion era. Am J Cardiol. 1998;82(9):1019–23.
    1. Physicians for Human Rights. The right to equal treatment. Available at . Cited 12 Nov 2005.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016004211.x', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016004211.x'}, {'type': 'PMC', 'value': 'PMC1495199', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmc1495199/'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11318921', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11318921/'}]}
    2. Weisse CS, Sorum PC, Sanders KN, Syat BL. Do gender and race affect decisions about pain management? J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(4):211–7.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '15328937', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15328937/'}]}
    2. Fincher C, Williams JE, MacLean V, Allison JJ, Kiefe CI, Canto JG. Racial disparities in coronary heart disease: a sociological view of the medical literature on physician bias. Ethn Dis. 2004;14(3):360–71.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1056/NEJM199911253412206', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1056/nejm199911253412206'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10572155', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10572155/'}]}
    2. Ayanian JZ, Cleary PD, Weissman JS, Epstein AM. The effect of patients’ preferences on racial differences in access to renal transplantation. N Engl J Med. 1999;341(22):1661–9.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11206944', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11206944/'}]}
    2. Bogart LM, Catz SL, Kelly JA, Benotsch EG. Factors influencing physicians’ judgments of adherence and treatment decisions for patients with HIV disease. Med Decis Making. 2001;21(1):28–36.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11789627', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11789627/'}]}
    2. van Ryn M. Research on the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in medical care. Med Care. 2002;40(1 suppl):I140–51.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00338-X', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00338-x'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10695979', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10695979/'}]}
    2. van Ryn M, Burke J. The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50(6):813–28.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1056/NEJM199902253400806', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1056/nejm199902253400806'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10029647', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10029647/'}]}
    2. Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(8):618–26.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5'}]}
    2. Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56(1):5–18.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PMC', 'value': 'PMC1447725', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmc1447725/'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '12554578', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12554578/'}]}
    2. van Ryn M, Fu SS. Paved with good intentions: do public health and human service providers contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in health? Am J Public Health. 2003;93(2):248–55.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1177/107755800773743646', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1177/107755800773743646'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11092162', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11092162/'}]}
    2. Einbinder LC, Schulman KA. The effect of race on the referral process for invasive cardiac procedures. Med Care Res Rev. 2000;57(suppl 1):162–80.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '9654756', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9654756/'}]}
    2. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit social cognition: the implicit association test. Am J Public Health. 1998;74(6):1464–80.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '8531054', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8531054/'}]}
    2. Fazio RH, Jackson JR, Dunton BC, Williams C. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(6):1013–27.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '12916565', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12916565/'}]}
    2. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the implicit association test: an improved scoring algorithm. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):197–216.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '7878162', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7878162/'}]}
    2. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self–esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev. 1995;102(1):4–27.
    1. None
    2. Banaji MR. Implicit attitudes can be measured. In: Roediger H, III, Nairne J, Neath I, Surprenant A, eds. The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G Crowder. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2001:117–50.
    1. Poehlman TA, Uhlmann E, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity.
    1. None
    2. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavior sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum; 1988.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101'}]}
    2. Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn. 2002;6(1):101–15.
    1. None
    2. Betancourt J. Not me! Doctors, decisions, and disparities in health care. Cardiovas Rev Rep. 2004;25(3):105–9.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/1099-9809.8.2.88', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.8.2.88'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11987594', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11987594/'}]}
    2. Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, Kawakami K, Hodson G. Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal biases and interracial distrust. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2002;8(2):88–102.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel