A balance of activity in brain control and reward systems predicts self-regulatory outcomes

Richard B Lopez, Pin-Hao A Chen, Jeremy F Huckins, Wilhelm Hofmann, William M Kelley, Todd F Heatherton, Richard B Lopez, Pin-Hao A Chen, Jeremy F Huckins, Wilhelm Hofmann, William M Kelley, Todd F Heatherton

Abstract

Previous neuroimaging work has shown that increased reward-related activity following exposure to food cues is predictive of self-control failure. The balance model suggests that self-regulation failures result from an imbalance in reward and executive control mechanisms. However, an open question is whether the relative balance of activity in brain systems associated with executive control (vs reward) supports self-regulatory outcomes when people encounter tempting cues in daily life. Sixty-nine chronic dieters, a population known for frequent lapses in self-control, completed a food cue-reactivity task during an fMRI scanning session, followed by a weeklong sampling of daily eating behaviors via ecological momentary assessment. We related participants' food cue activity in brain systems associated with executive control and reward to real-world eating patterns. Specifically, a balance score representing the amount of activity in brain regions associated with self-regulatory control, relative to automatic reward-related activity, predicted dieters' control over their eating behavior during the following week. This balance measure may reflect individual self-control capacity and be useful for examining self-regulation success in other domains and populations.

Keywords: dieting; fMRI; individual differences; reward; self-control.

© The Author (2017). Published by Oxford University Press.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Lateral (top left and right) and ventral (bottom) views on an inflated brain surface depicting ROIs in FP (yellow) and reward (green) systems. ROI spheres have been enlarged to 10 mm for better visualization.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Scatter plot of regulation-reward balance scores regressed on percentage of enacted food desires.

References

    1. Aarts H. (2007). On the emergence of human goal pursuit: The nonconscious regulation and motivation of goals. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 183–201.
    1. Aron A.R., Robbins T.W., Poldrack R.A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 177–85.
    1. Berkman E.T., Falk E.B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: Using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 45–50.
    1. Berkman E.T., Falk E.B., Lieberman M.D. (2011). In the trenches of real-world self-control: neural correlates of breaking the link between craving and smoking. Psychological Science, 22(4), 498–506.
    1. Berkman E.T., Kahn L.E., Merchant J.S. (2014). Training-induced changes in inhibitory control network activity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(1), 149–57.
    1. Casey B.J., Somerville L.H., Gotlib I.H., et al. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(36), 14998–5003.
    1. Cloutier J., Heatherton T.F., Whalen P.J., Kelley W.M. (2008). Are attractive people rewarding? sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(6), 941–51.
    1. Demos K.E., Heatherton T.F., Kelley W.M. (2012). Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(16), 5549–52.
    1. Demos K.E., Kelley W.M., Heatherton T.F. (2011). Dietary restraint violations influence reward responses in nucleus accumbens and amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(8), 1952–63.
    1. Diekhof E.K., Kaps L., Falkai P., Gruber O. (2012). The role of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbitofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magnitude–an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancy and outcome processing. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1252–66.
    1. Donaldson M.S. (2004). Nutrition and cancer: a review of the evidence for an anti-cancer diet. Nutrition Journal, 3, (19), 1–21.
    1. Dosenbach N.U.F., Fair D.A., Miezin F.M., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(26), 11073–8.
    1. Freeman S.M., Aron A.R. (2016). Withholding a reward-driven action: studies of the rise and fall of motor activation and the effect of cognitive depletion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(2), 237–51.
    1. Garavan H., Pankiewicz J., Bloom A., et al. (2000). Cue-induced cocaine craving: Neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(11), 1789–98.
    1. Giuliani N.R., Tomiyama A.J., Mann T., Berkman E.T. (2015). Prediction of daily food intake as a function of measurement modality and restriction status. Psychosomatic Medicine, 77(5), 583–90.
    1. Gorin A.A., Stone A.A. (2001). Recall biases and cognitive errors in retrospective self-reports: A call for momentary assessments. Handbook of Health Psychology, 23, 405–13.
    1. Heatherton T.F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363.
    1. Heatherton T.F., Baumeister R.F. (1991). Binge eating as an escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108.
    1. Heatherton T.F., Herman C.P., Polivy J., King G.A., McGree S.T. (1988). The (mis)measurement of restraint: An analysis of conceptual and psychometric issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(1), 19–28.
    1. Heatherton T.F., Wagner D.D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132–9.
    1. Herman C.P., Polivy J. (1975). Anxiety, restraint, and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84(6), 666–72.
    1. Herman C.P., Polivy J. (1980). Restrained eating In:Stunkard A.J.editor.Obesity. Philadelphia: Saunders.
    1. Hofmann W., Baumeister R.F., Förster G., Vohs K.D. (2012). Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1318–35.
    1. Hofmann W., Friese M., Strack F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2), 162–76.
    1. Hofmann W., Patel P.V. (2015). SurveySignal: a convenient solution for experience sampling research using participants’ own smartphones. Social Science Computer Review, 33(2), 235–53.
    1. Holm-Denoma J.M., Joiner T.E. Jr., Vohs K.D., Heatherton T.F. (2008). The “freshman fifteen” (the “freshman five” actually): predictors and possible explanations. Health Psychology, 27(1S), S3–9.
    1. James W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 2 Mineola, NY: Dover.
    1. Kelley W.M., Wagner D.D., Heatherton T.F. (2015). In search of a human self-regulation system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 389–411.
    1. Knutson B., Taylor J., Kaufman M., Peterson R., Glover G. (2005). Distributed neural representation of expected value. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(19), 4806–12.
    1. Kober H., Mende-Siedlecki P., Kross E.F., et al. (2010). Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 14811–6.
    1. Kotabe H.P., Hofmann W. (2015). On integrating the components of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 618–38.
    1. Lewin K. (1951). Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers. Oxford, England: Harper.
    1. Lopez R.B., Hofmann W., Wagner D.D., Kelley W.M., Heatherton T.F. (2014). Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1337–44.
    1. Martin G.S., Tapsell L.C., Batterham M.J., Russell K.G. (2002). Relative bias in diet history measurements: a quality control technique for dietary intervention trials. Public Health Nutrition, 5(4), 537–46.
    1. Neal D.T., Wood W., Labrecque J.S., Lally P. (2012). How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 492–8.
    1. Padmala S., Pessoa L. (2010). Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 558–65.
    1. Petersen S.E., Posner M.I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73.
    1. Power J.D., Cohen A.L., Nelson S.M., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665–78.
    1. Rosvall M., Bergstrom C.T. (2008). Maps of random walks on complex networks reveal community structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(4), 1118–23.
    1. Somerville L.H., Hare T., Casey B.J. (2010). Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2123–34.
    1. Stone A.A., Shiffman S. (2002). Capturing momentary, self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 24(3), 236–43.
    1. Tomiyama A.J., Mann T., Vinas D., Hunger J.M., DeJager J., Taylor S.E. (2010). Low calorie dieting increases cortisol. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(4), 357–64.
    1. van der Laan L.N., de Ridder D.T.D., Viergever M.A., Smeets P.A.M. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: A meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage, 55(1), 296–303.
    1. Wagner D.D., Altman M., Boswell R.G., Kelley W.M., Heatherton T.F. (2013). Self-regulatory depletion enhances neural responses to rewards and impairs top-down control. Psychological Science, 2262–71.
    1. Wagner D.D., Boswell R.G., Kelley W.M., Heatherton T.F. (2012). Inducing negative affect increases the reward value of appetizing foods in dieters. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(7), 1625–33.
    1. Wagner D.D., Heatherton T.F. (2015). Self-regulation and its failure: The seven deadly threats to self-regulation In: Mikulincer M., Shaver P.R., Borgida E., Bargh J.A., editors. APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, pp. 805–42. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
    1. Wardle J., Haase A.M., Steptoe A., Nillapun M., Jonwutiwes K., Bellisie F. (2004). Gender differences in food choice: the contribution of health beliefs and dieting. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27(2), 107–16.
    1. White C.N., Congdon E., Mumford J.A., et al. (2014). Decomposing decision components in the stop-signal task: a model-based approach to individual differences in inhibitory control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1601–14.
    1. Wood W., Rünger D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 289–314.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel