Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers

Amit Lampit, Harry Hallock, Michael Valenzuela, Amit Lampit, Harry Hallock, Michael Valenzuela

Abstract

Background: New effective interventions to attenuate age-related cognitive decline are a global priority. Computerized cognitive training (CCT) is believed to be safe and can be inexpensive, but neither its efficacy in enhancing cognitive performance in healthy older adults nor the impact of design factors on such efficacy has been systematically analyzed. Our aim therefore was to quantitatively assess whether CCT programs can enhance cognition in healthy older adults, discriminate responsive from nonresponsive cognitive domains, and identify the most salient design factors.

Methods and findings: We systematically searched Medline, Embase, and PsycINFO for relevant studies from the databases' inception to 9 July 2014. Eligible studies were randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of ≥ 4 h of CCT on performance in neuropsychological tests in older adults without dementia or other cognitive impairment. Fifty-two studies encompassing 4,885 participants were eligible. Intervention designs varied considerably, but after removal of one outlier, heterogeneity across studies was small (I(2) = 29.92%). There was no systematic evidence of publication bias. The overall effect size (Hedges' g, random effects model) for CCT versus control was small and statistically significant, g = 0.22 (95% CI 0.15 to 0.29). Small to moderate effect sizes were found for nonverbal memory, g = 0.24 (95% CI 0.09 to 0.38); verbal memory, g = 0.08 (95% CI 0.01 to 0.15); working memory (WM), g = 0.22 (95% CI 0.09 to 0.35); processing speed, g = 0.31 (95% CI 0.11 to 0.50); and visuospatial skills, g = 0.30 (95% CI 0.07 to 0.54). No significant effects were found for executive functions and attention. Moderator analyses revealed that home-based administration was ineffective compared to group-based training, and that more than three training sessions per week was ineffective versus three or fewer. There was no evidence for the effectiveness of WM training, and only weak evidence for sessions less than 30 min. These results are limited to healthy older adults, and do not address the durability of training effects.

Conclusions: CCT is modestly effective at improving cognitive performance in healthy older adults, but efficacy varies across cognitive domains and is largely determined by design choices. Unsupervised at-home training and training more than three times per week are specifically ineffective. Further research is required to enhance efficacy of the intervention. Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary.

Conflict of interest statement

MV has received research funding and honoraria from the Brain Department LLC for a project unrelated to this work. His group also receives in-kind research support in the form of no-cost software from BrainTrain Inc (USA), also unrelated to this work.

Figures

Figure 1. Summary of trial identification and…
Figure 1. Summary of trial identification and selection.
Note that a single study could be excluded on more than one criterion, but appears only once in the chart.
Figure 2. Overall efficacy of CCT on…
Figure 2. Overall efficacy of CCT on all cognitive outcomes.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 3. Funnel plot for overall effects…
Figure 3. Funnel plot for overall effects after removal of one outlier .
Figure 4. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 4. Efficacy of CCT on measures of verbal memory.
Effect estimates are based on fixed-effects (top) and random-effects (bottom) models, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 5. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 5. Efficacy of CCT on measures of nonverbal memory.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 6. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 6. Efficacy of CCT on measures of working memory.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 7. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 7. Efficacy of CCT on measures of processing speed.
Effect estimates are based on fixed-effects (top) and random-effects (bottom) models, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 8. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 8. Efficacy of CCT on measures of executive functions.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 9. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 9. Efficacy of CCT on measures of attention.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 10. Efficacy of CCT on measures…
Figure 10. Efficacy of CCT on measures of visuospatial skills.
Effect estimates are based on a random-effects model, and studies are rank-ordered by year of publication.
Figure 11. Subgroup analyses of moderators of…
Figure 11. Subgroup analyses of moderators of overall efficacy of CCT in older adults.
aQ-test for between-group heterogeneity, mixed-effects model. bOne study that combined data from both home- and group-based training was excluded from this analysis. cTotal number of training hours. dSession length could not be determined for one study.
Figure 12. Overview of efficacy and moderators…
Figure 12. Overview of efficacy and moderators of efficacy for CCT in older adults.
Numbers refer to SMDs from an individual meta-analysis (see Figures S2, S3, S4, S5, S6, S7, S8 for details). Colored cells indicate significant outcomes, with effect sizes color coded: yellow, g<0.3; pink, g = 0.3–0.6; red, g≥0.6. White depicts non-significant results, and grey shows where no studies were available for analysis. *p<0.05, **p<0.01 for within-subgroup results (between-subgroup results are reported in Figures 11 and S2, S3, S4, S5, S6, S7, S8). aBased on a fixed-effects model because of evidence of potential publication bias in these outcomes. bSMD based on a single trial.

References

    1. Deary IJ, Corley J, Gow AJ, Harris SE, Houlihan LM, et al. (2009) Age-associated cognitive decline. Br Med Bull 92: 135–152.
    1. Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, Eastwood R, Gauthier S, et al. (1997) Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly population. Lancet 349: 1793–1796.
    1. Plassman BL, Langa KM, McCammon RJ, Fisher GG, Potter GG, et al. (2011) Incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment, not dementia in the United States. Ann Neurol 70: 418–426.
    1. Unverzagt FW, Gao S, Baiyewu O, Ogunniyi AO, Gureje O, et al. (2001) Prevalence of cognitive impairment: data from the Indianapolis Study of Health and Aging. Neurology 57: 1655–1662.
    1. Zhu CW, Sano M, Ferris SH, Whitehouse PJ, Patterson MB, et al. (2013) Health-related resource use and costs in elderly adults with and without mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 61: 396–402.
    1. Marioni RE, Valenzuela MJ, van den Hout A, Brayne C, Matthews FE, et al. (2012) Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS ONE 7: e50940.
    1. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, et al. (2003) Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 348: 2508–2516.
    1. Wilson RS, Mendes De Leon CF, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bienias JL, et al. (2002) Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287: 742–748.
    1. Kueider AM, Parisi JM, Gross AL, Rebok GW (2012) Computerized cognitive training with older adults: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 7: e40588.
    1. Clare L, Woods RT, Moniz Cook ED, Orrell M, Spector A (2003) Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2003: CD003260.
    1. Jak A, Seelye A, Jurick S (2013) Crosswords to computers: a critical review of popular approaches to cognitive enhancement. Neuropsychol Rev 23: 13–26.
    1. Commercialising neuroscience: Brain sells. The Economist. Available: . Accessed 11 November 2014.
    1. Green CS, Strobach T, Schubert T (2013) On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychol Res 10.1007/s00426-013-0535-3
    1. Valenzuela M, Sachdev P (2009) Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 179–187.
    1. Papp KV, Walsh SJ, Snyder PJ (2009) Immediate and delayed effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: a review of current literature and future directions. Alzheimers Dement 5: 50–60.
    1. Martin M, Clare L, Altgassen AM, Cameron MH, Zehnder F (2011) Cognition-based interventions for healthy older people and people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011: CD006220.
    1. Reijnders J, van Heugten C, van Boxtel M (2013) Cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Ageing Res Rev 12: 263–275.
    1. Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, et al. (2014) The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev 15: 28–43.
    1. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, et al. (2009) The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med 6: e1000100.
    1. Ward A, Arrighi HM, Michels S, Cedarbaum JM (2012) Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement 8: 14–21.
    1. Strauss EH, Sherman EMS, Spreen OA, editors (2006) A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms and commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji F, et al. (2013) Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501: 97–101.
    1. Maillot P, Perrot A, Hartley A (2012) Effects of interactive physical-activity video-game training on physical and cognitive function in older adults. Psychol Aging 27: 589–600.
    1. Nouchi R, Taki Y, Takeuchi H, Hashizume H, Akitsuki Y, et al. (2012) Brain training game improves executive functions and processing speed in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 7: e29676.
    1. Smith GE, Housen P, Yaffe K, Ruff R, Kennison RF, et al. (2009) A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study. J Am Geriatr Soc 57: 594–603.
    1. Bavelier D, Green CS, Han DH, Renshaw PF, Merzenich MM, et al. (2011) Brains on video games. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 763–768.
    1. Higgins J, Green S, editors (2011) Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1.0. The Cochrane Collaboration.
    1. Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M (2003) Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther 83: 713–721.
    1. Wykes T, Huddy V, Cellard C, McGurk SR, Czobor P (2011) A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. Am J Psychiatry 168: 472–485.
    1. Gleser LJ, Olkin I (2009) Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In: Cooper H, Hedges L, Valentine J, editors. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis, 2nd edition. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. pp. 357–376.
    1. Borenstein M, Hedges L, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR (2009) Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.
    1. Higgins JP, Thompson SG (2002) Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 21: 1539–1558.
    1. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG (2003) Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327: 557–560.
    1. Borenstein M, Higgins JP (2013) Meta-analysis and subgroups. Prev Sci 14: 134–143.
    1. Sterne JA, Sutton AJ, Ioannidis JP, Terrin N, Jones DR, et al. (2011) Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. BMJ 343: d4002.
    1. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C (1997) Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315: 629–634.
    1. McDougall S, House B (2012) Brain training in older adults: evidence of transfer to memory span performance and pseudo-Matthew effects. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 19: 195–221.
    1. Schmiedek F, Lovden M, Lindenberger U (2010) Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: findings from the COGITO study. Front Aging Neurosci 2: 27.
    1. Theill N, Schumacher V, Adelsberger R, Martin M, Jancke L (2013) Effects of simultaneously performed cognitive and physical training in older adults. BMC Neurosci 14: 103.
    1. Gajewski PD, Falkenstein M (2012) Training-induced improvement of response selection and error detection in aging assessed by task switching: effects of cognitive, physical, and relaxation training. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 130.
    1. Wild-Wall N, Falkenstein M, Gajewski PD (2012) Neural correlates of changes in a visual search task due to cognitive training in seniors. Neural Plast 2012: 529057.
    1. Barnes DE, Santos-Modesitt W, Poelke G, Kramer AF, Castro C, et al. (2013) The Mental Activity and eXercise (MAX) trial: a randomized controlled trial to enhance cognitive function in older adults. JAMA Intern Med 173: 797–804.
    1. Colzato LS, van Muijden J, Band GP, Hommel B (2011) Genetic modulation of training and transfer in older adults: BDNF ValMet polymorphism is associated with wider useful field of view. Front Psychol 2: 199.
    1. Dahlin E, Nyberg L, Backman L, Neely AS (2008) Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol Aging 23: 720–730.
    1. Dustman RE, Emmerson RY, Steinhaus LA, Shearer DE, Dustman TJ (1992) The effects of videogame playing on neuropsychological performance of elderly individuals. J Gerontol 47: P168–P171.
    1. Mahncke HW, Connor BB, Appelman J, Ahsanuddin ON, Hardy JL, et al. (2006) Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: a randomized, controlled study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 12523–12528.
    1. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Vance DE, Wood K, Roenker DL, et al. (2005) The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health 9: 262–271.
    1. Goldstein J, Cajko L, Oosterbroek M, Michielsen M, Van Houten O, et al. (1997) Video games and the elderly. Soc Behav Pers 25: 345–352.
    1. Lampit A, Hallock H, Moss R, Kwok S, Rosser M, et al. (2014) The timecourse of global cognitive gains from supervised computer-assisted cognitive training: a randomised, active-controlled trial in elderly with multiple dementia risk factors. J Prev Alzheimers Dis 1: 33–39.
    1. Lee Y, Lee C-R, Hwang B (2012) Effects of computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation training and balance exercise on cognitive and visual perception ability of the elderly. J Phys Ther Sci 24: 885–887.
    1. Vance D, Dawson J, Wadley V, Edwards J, Roenker D, et al. (2007) The accelerate study: the longitudinal effect of speed of processing training on cognitive performance of older adults. Rehabil Psychol 52: 89–96.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Howren MB, Jones MP, Martin R, et al. (2011) Interim analyses from a randomised controlled trial to improve visual processing speed in older adults: the Iowa Healthy and Active Minds Study. BMJ Open 1: e000225.
    1. Belchior P, Marsiske M, Sisco SM, Yam A, Bavelier D, et al. (2013) Video game training to improve selective visual attention in older adults. Comput Human Behav 29: 1318–1324.
    1. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, et al. (2002) Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288: 2271–2281.
    1. Berry AS, Zanto TP, Clapp WC, Hardy JL, Delahunt PB, et al. (2010) The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older adults. PLoS ONE 5: e11537.
    1. Bottiroli S, Cavallini E (2009) Can computer familiarity regulate the benefits of computer-based memory training in normal aging? A study with an Italian sample of older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 16: 401–418.
    1. Garcia-Campuzano MT, Virues-Ortega J, Smith S, Moussavi Z (2013) Effect of cognitive training targeting associative memory in the elderly: a small randomized trial and a longitudinal evaluation. J Am Geriatr Soc 61: 2252–2254.
    1. Li KZ, Roudaia E, Lussier M, Bherer L, Leroux A, et al. (2010) Benefits of cognitive dual-task training on balance performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 65: 1344–1352.
    1. Lussier M, Gagnon C, Bherer L (2012) An investigation of response and stimulus modality transfer effects after dual-task training in younger and older. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 129.
    1. Mayas J, Parmentier FBR, Andres P, Ballesteros S (2014) Plasticity of attentional functions in older adults after non-action video game training: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 9: e92269.
    1. O'Brien JL, Edwards JD, Maxfield ND, Peronto CL, Williams VA, et al. (2013) Cognitive training and selective attention in the aging brain: an electrophysiological study. Clin Neurophysiol 124: 2198–2208.
    1. Peng H, Wen J, Wang D, Gao Y (2012) The impact of processing speed training on working memory in old adults. J Adult Dev 19: 150–157.
    1. van Muijden J, Band GP, Hommel B (2012) Online games training aging brains: limited transfer to cognitive control functions. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 221.
    1. Ackerman PL, Kanfer R, Calderwood C (2010) Use it or lose it? Wii brain exercise practice and reading for domain knowledge. Psychol Aging 25: 753–766.
    1. Anderson S, White-Schwoch T, Parbery-Clark A, Kraus N (2013) Reversal of age-related neural timing delays with training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: 4357–4362.
    1. Basak C, Boot WR, Voss MW, Kramer AF (2008) Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol Aging 23: 765–777.
    1. Boot WR, Champion M, Blakely DP, Wright T, Souders DJ, et al. (2013) Video games as a means to reduce age-related cognitive decline: attitudes, compliance, and effectiveness. Front Psychol 4: 31.
    1. Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC (2013) Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 57: 1–7.
    1. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, Cissell GM, et al. (2002) Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology 48: 329–340.
    1. Edwards JD, Valdes EG, Peronto C, Castora-Binkley M, Alwerdt J, et al. (2013) The efficacy of InSight cognitive training to improve useful field of view performance: a brief report. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 10.1093/geronb/gbt113
    1. Legault C, Jennings JM, Katula JA, Dagenbach D, Gaussoin SA, et al. (2011) Designing clinical trials for assessing the effects of cognitive training and physical activity interventions on cognitive outcomes: the Seniors Health and Activity Research Program Pilot (SHARP-P) study, a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr 11: 27.
    1. Miller KJ, Dye RV, Kim J, Jennings JL, O'Toole E, et al. (2013) Effect of a computerized brain exercise program on cognitive performance in older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 21: 655–663.
    1. Rasmusson DX, Rebok GW, Bylsma FW, Brandt J (1999) Effects of three types of memory training in normal elderly. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 6: 56–66.
    1. Richmond LL, Morrison AB, Chein JM, Olson IR (2011) Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol Aging 26: 813–822.
    1. Shatil E (2013) Does combined cognitive training and physical activity training enhance cognitive abilities more than either alone? A four-condition randomized controlled trial among healthy older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 5: 8.
    1. Stern Y, Blumen HM, Rich LW, Richards A, Herzberg G, et al. (2011) Space Fortress game training and executive control in older adults: a pilot intervention. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 18: 653–677.
    1. Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Backman L (2012) Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 63.
    1. Burki CN, Ludwig C, Chicherio C, de Ribaupierre A (2014) Individual differences in cognitive plasticity: an investigation of training curves in younger and older adults. Psychol Res 10.1007/s00426-014-0559-3
    1. Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM, Hutchison S, Perrig-Chiello P, Dapp C, et al. (2008) Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychol Aging 23: 743–753.
    1. Casutt G, Theill N, Martin M, Keller M, Jancke L (2014) The drive-wise project: driving simulator training increases real driving performance in healthy older drivers. Front Aging Neurosci 6: 85.
    1. Heinzel S, Schulte S, Onken J, Duong QL, Riemer TG, et al. (2014) Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 21: 146–173.
    1. McAvinue LP, Golemme M, Castorina M, Tatti E, Pigni FM, et al. (2013) An evaluation of a working memory training scheme in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 5: 20.
    1. Sandberg P, Ronnlund M, Nyberg L, Stigsdotter Neely A (2014) Executive process training in young and old adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 21: 577–605.
    1. Shatil E, Mikulecka J, Bellotti F, Bures V (2014) Novel television-based cognitive training improves working memory and executive function. PLoS ONE 9: e101472.
    1. von Bastian CC, Langer N, Jancke L, Oberauer K (2013) Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Mem Cognit 41: 611–624.
    1. Simpson T, Camfield D, Pipingas A, Macpherson H, Stough C (2012) Improved processing speed: online computer-based cognitive training in older adults. Educ Gerontol 38: 445–458.
    1. Peretz C, Korczyn AD, Shatil E, Aharonson V, Birnboim S, et al. (2011) Computer-based, personalized cognitive training versus classical computer games: a randomized double-blind prospective trial of cognitive stimulation. Neuroepidemiology 36: 91–99.
    1. Wang MY, Chang CY, Su SY (2011) What's cooking?—cognitive training of executive function in the elderly. Front Psychol 2: 228.
    1. Colcombe S, Kramer AF (2003) Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 14: 125–130.
    1. Gross AL, Parisi JM, Spira AP, Kueider AM, Ko JY, et al. (2012) Memory training interventions for older adults: a meta-analysis. Aging Ment Health 16: 722–734.
    1. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C (2013) Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol 49: 270–291.
    1. Kivipelto M, Solomon A, Ahtiluoto S, Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, et al. (2013) The Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER): study design and progress. Alzheimers Dement 9: 657–665.
    1. Anstey KJ, Burns RA, Birrell CL, Steel D, Kiely KM, et al. (2010) Estimates of probable dementia prevalence from population-based surveys compared with dementia prevalence estimates based on meta-analyses. BMC Neurol 10: 62.
    1. Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L (1992) Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging 7: 242–251.
    1. Luscher C, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC, Muller D (2000) Synaptic plasticity and dynamic modulation of the postsynaptic membrane. Nat Neurosci 3: 545–550.
    1. Holtzer R, Shuman M, Mahoney JR, Lipton R, Verghese J (2011) Cognitive fatigue defined in the context of attention networks. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 18: 108–128.
    1. Wang Z, Zhou R, Shah P (2014) Spaced cognitive training promotes training transfer. Front Hum Neurosci 8: 217.
    1. Penner IK, Vogt A, Stcklin M, Gschwind L, Opwis K, et al. (2012) Computerised working memory training in healthy adults: a comparison of two different training schedules. Neuropsychol Rehabil 22: 716–733.
    1. Gates NJ, Sachdev PS, Fiatarone Singh MA, Valenzuela M (2011) Cognitive and memory training in adults at risk of dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 11: 55.
    1. Bahar-Fuchs A, Clare L, Woods B (2013) Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 6: CD003260.
    1. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Shah P (2011) Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 10081–10086.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel